<p>ON THE RELATION BETWEEN THE PERIOD OF DEVELOPMENT OF A CHARACTER AND ITS
TRANSMISSION TO ONE SEX OR TO BOTH SEXES.</p>
<p>Why certain characters should be inherited by both sexes, and other characters
by one sex alone, namely by that sex in which the character first appeared, is
in most cases quite unknown. We cannot even conjecture why with certain
sub-breeds of the pigeon, black striae, though transmitted through the female,
should be developed in the male alone, whilst every other character is equally
transferred to both sexes. Why, again, with cats, the tortoise-shell colour
should, with rare exceptions, be developed in the female alone. The very same
character, such as deficient or supernumerary digits, colour-blindness, etc.,
may with mankind be inherited by the males alone of one family, and in another
family by the females alone, though in both cases transmitted through the
opposite as well as through the same sex. (38. References are given in my
‘Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,’ vol. ii. p.
72.) Although we are thus ignorant, the two following rules seem often to hold
good—that variations which first appear in either sex at a late period of
life, tend to be developed in the same sex alone; whilst variations which first
appear early in life in either sex tend to be developed in both sexes. I am,
however, far from supposing that this is the sole determining cause. As I have
not elsewhere discussed this subject, and it has an important bearing on sexual
selection, I must here enter into lengthy and somewhat intricate details.</p>
<p>It is in itself probable that any character appearing at an early age would
tend to be inherited equally by both sexes, for the sexes do not differ much in
constitution before the power of reproduction is gained. On the other hand,
after this power has been gained and the sexes have come to differ in
constitution, the gemmules (if I may again use the language of pangenesis)
which are cast off from each varying part in the one sex would be much more
likely to possess the proper affinities for uniting with the tissues of the
same sex, and thus becoming developed, than with those of the opposite sex.</p>
<p>I was first led to infer that a relation of this kind exists, from the fact
that whenever and in whatever manner the adult male differs from the adult
female, he differs in the same manner from the young of both sexes. The
generality of this fact is quite remarkable: it holds good with almost all
mammals, birds, amphibians, and fishes; also with many crustaceans, spiders,
and some few insects, such as certain orthoptera and libellulae. In all these
cases the variations, through the accumulation of which the male acquired his
proper masculine characters, must have occurred at a somewhat late period of
life; otherwise the young males would have been similarly characterised; and
conformably with our rule, the variations are transmitted to and developed in
the adult males alone. When, on the other hand, the adult male closely
resembles the young of both sexes (these, with rare exceptions, being alike),
he generally resembles the adult female; and in most of these cases the
variations through which the young and old acquired their present characters,
probably occurred, according to our rule, during youth. But there is here room
for doubt, for characters are sometimes transferred to the offspring at an
earlier age than that at which they first appeared in the parents, so that the
parents may have varied when adult, and have transferred their characters to
their offspring whilst young. There are, moreover, many animals, in which the
two sexes closely resemble each other, and yet both differ from their young:
and here the characters of the adults must have been acquired late in life;
nevertheless, these characters, in apparent contradiction to our rule, are
transferred to both sexes. We must not however, overlook the possibility or
even probability of successive variations of the same nature occurring, under
exposure to similar conditions, simultaneously in both sexes at a rather late
period of life; and in this case the variations would be transferred to the
offspring of both sexes at a corresponding late age; and there would then be no
real contradiction to the rule that variations occurring late in life are
transferred exclusively to the sex in which they first appeared. This latter
rule seems to hold true more generally than the second one, namely, that
variations which occur in either sex early in life tend to be transferred to
both sexes. As it was obviously impossible even to estimate in how large a
number of cases throughout the animal kingdom these two propositions held good,
it occurred to me to investigate some striking or crucial instances, and to
rely on the result.</p>
<p>An excellent case for investigation is afforded by the Deer family. In all the
species, but one, the horns are developed only in the males, though certainly
transmitted through the females, and capable of abnormal development in them.
In the reindeer, on the other hand, the female is provided with horns; so that
in this species, the horns ought, according to our rule, to appear early in
life, long before the two sexes are mature and have come to differ much in
constitution. In all the other species the horns ought to appear later in life,
which would lead to their development in that sex alone, in which they first
appeared in the progenitor of the whole Family. Now in seven species, belonging
to distinct sections of the family and inhabiting different regions, in which
the stags alone bear horns, I find that the horns first appear at periods,
varying from nine months after birth in the roebuck, to ten, twelve or even
more months in the stags of the six other and larger species. (39. I am much
obliged to Mr. Cupples for having made enquiries for me in regard to the
Roebuck and Red Deer of Scotland from Mr. Robertson, the experienced
head-forester to the Marquis of Breadalbane. In regard to Fallow-deer, I have
to thank Mr. Eyton and others for information. For the Cervus alces of N.
America, see ‘Land and Water,’ 1868, pp. 221 and 254; and for the
C. Virginianus and strongyloceros of the same continent, see J.D. Caton, in
‘Ottawa Acad. of Nat. Sc.’ 1868, p. 13. For Cervus Eldi of Pegu,
see Lieut. Beaven, ‘Proccedings of the Zoological Society,’ 1867,
p. 762.) But with the reindeer the case is widely different; for, as I hear
from Prof. Nilsson, who kindly made special enquiries for me in Lapland, the
horns appear in the young animals within four or five weeks after birth, and at
the same time in both sexes. So that here we have a structure, developed at a
most unusually early age in one species of the family, and likewise common to
both sexes in this one species alone.</p>
<p>In several kinds of antelopes, only the males are provided with horns, whilst
in the greater number both sexes bear horns. With respect to the period of
development, Mr. Blyth informs me that there was at one time in the Zoological
Gardens a young koodoo (Ant. strepsiceros), of which the males alone are
horned, and also the young of a closely-allied species, the eland (Ant. oreas),
in which both sexes are horned. Now it is in strict conformity with our rule,
that in the young male koodoo, although ten months old, the horns were
remarkably small, considering the size ultimately attained by them; whilst in
the young male eland, although only three months old, the horns were already
very much larger than in the koodoo. It is also a noticeable fact that in the
prong-horned antelope (40. Antilocapra Americana. I have to thank Dr. Canfield
for information with respect to the horns of the female: see also his paper in
‘Proceedings of the Zoological Society,’ 1866, p. 109. Also Owen,
‘Anatomy of Vertebrates,’ vol. iii. p. 627), only a few of the
females, about one in five, have horns, and these are in a rudimentary state,
though sometimes above four inches long: so that as far as concerns the
possession of horns by the males alone, this species is in an intermediate
condition, and the horns do not appear until about five or six months after
birth. Therefore in comparison with what little we know of the development of
the horns in other antelopes, and from what we do know with respect to the
horns of deer, cattle, etc., those of the prong-horned antelope appear at an
intermediate period of life,—that is, not very early, as in cattle and
sheep, nor very late, as in the larger deer and antelopes. The horns of sheep,
goats, and cattle, which are well developed in both sexes, though not quite
equal in size, can be felt, or even seen, at birth or soon afterwards. (41. I
have been assured that the horns of the sheep in North Wales can always be
felt, and are sometimes even an inch in length, at birth. Youatt says
(‘Cattle,’ 1834, p. 277), that the prominence of the frontal bone
in cattle penetrates the cutis at birth, and that the horny matter is soon
formed over it.) Our rule, however, seems to fail in some breeds of sheep, for
instance merinos, in which the rams alone are horned; for I cannot find on
enquiry (42. I am greatly indebted to Prof. Victor Carus for having made
enquiries for me, from the highest authorities, with respect to the merino
sheep of Saxony. On the Guinea coast of Africa there is, however, a breed of
sheep in which, as with merinos, the rams alone bear horns; and Mr. Winwood
Reade informs me that in one case observed by him, a young ram, born on Feb.
10th, first shewed horns on March 6th, so that in this instance, in conformity
with rule, the development of the horns occurred at a later period of life than
in Welsh sheep, in which both sexes are horned.), that the horns are developed
later in life in this breed than in ordinary sheep in which both sexes are
horned. But with domesticated sheep the presence or absence of horns is not a
firmly fixed character; for a certain proportion of the merino ewes bear small
horns, and some of the rams are hornless; and in most breeds hornless ewes are
occasionally produced.</p>
<p>Dr. W. Marshall has lately made a special study of the protuberances so common
on the heads of birds (43. ‘Über die knochernen Schädelhöcker der
Vögel,’ in the ‘Niederland. Archiv fur Zoologie,’ B.i. Heft
2, 1872.), and he comes to the following conclusion:—that with those
species in which they are confined to the males, they are developed late in
life; whereas with those species in which they are common to the two sexes,
they are developed at a very early period. This is certainly a striking
confirmation of my two laws of inheritance.</p>
<p>In most of the species of the splendid family of the Pheasants, the males
differ conspicuously from the females, and they acquire their ornaments at a
rather late period of life. The eared pheasant (Crossoptilon auritum), however,
offers a remarkable exception, for both sexes possess the fine caudal plumes,
the large ear-tufts and the crimson velvet about the head; I find that all
these characters appear very early in life in accordance with rule. The adult
male can, however, be distinguished from the adult female by the presence of
spurs; and conformably with our rule, these do not begin to be developed before
the age of six months, as I am assured by Mr. Bartlett, and even at this age,
the two sexes can hardly be distinguished. (44. In the common peacock (Pavo
cristatus) the male alone possesses spurs, whilst both sexes of the Java
Peacock (P. muticus) offer the unusual case of being furnished with spurs.
Hence I fully expected that in the latter species they would have been
developed earlier in life than in the common peacock; but M. Hegt of Amsterdam
informs me, that with young birds of the previous year, of both species,
compared on April 23rd, 1869, there was no difference in the development of the
spurs. The spurs, however, were as yet represented merely by slight knobs or
elevations. I presume that I should have been informed if any difference in the
rate of development had been observed subsequently.) The male and female
Peacock differ conspicuously from each other in almost every part of their
plumage, except in the elegant head-crest, which is common to both sexes; and
this is developed very early in life, long before the other ornaments, which
are confined to the male. The wild-duck offers an analogous case, for the
beautiful green speculum on the wings is common to both sexes, though duller
and somewhat smaller in the female, and it is developed early in life, whilst
the curled tail-feathers and other ornaments of the male are developed later.
(45. In some other species of the Duck family the speculum differs in a greater
degree in the two sexes; but I have not been able to discover whether its full
development occurs later in life in the males of such species, than in the male
of the common duck, as ought to be the case according to our rule. With the
allied Mergus cucullatus we have, however, a case of this kind: the two sexes
differ conspicuously in general plumage, and to a considerable degree in the
speculum, which is pure white in the male and greyish-white in the female. Now
the young males at first entirely resemble the females, and have a
greyish-white speculum, which becomes pure white at an earlier age than that at
which the adult male acquires his other and more strongly-marked sexual
differences: see Audubon, ‘Ornithological Biography,’ vol. iii.
1835, pp. 249-250.) Between such extreme cases of close sexual resemblance and
wide dissimilarity, as those of the Crossoptilon and peacock, many intermediate
ones could be given, in which the characters follow our two rules in their
order of development.</p>
<p>As most insects emerge from the pupal state in a mature condition, it is
doubtful whether the period of development can determine the transference of
their characters to one or to both sexes. But we do not know that the coloured
scales, for instance, in two species of butterflies, in one of which the sexes
differ in colour, whilst in the other they are alike, are developed at the same
relative age in the cocoon. Nor do we know whether all the scales are
simultaneously developed on the wings of the same species of butterfly, in
which certain coloured marks are confined to one sex, whilst others are common
to both sexes. A difference of this kind in the period of development is not so
improbable as it may at first appear; for with the Orthoptera, which assume
their adult state, not by a single metamorphosis, but by a succession of
moults, the young males of some species at first resemble the females, and
acquire their distinctive masculine characters only at a later moult. Strictly
analogous cases occur at the successive moults of certain male crustaceans.</p>
<p>We have as yet considered the transference of characters, relatively to their
period of development, only in species in a natural state; we will now turn to
domesticated animals, and first touch on monstrosities and diseases. The
presence of supernumerary digits, and the absence of certain phalanges, must be
determined at an early embryonic period—the tendency to profuse bleeding
is at least congenital, as is probably colour-blindness—yet these
peculiarities, and other similar ones, are often limited in their transmission
to one sex; so that the rule that characters, developed at an early period,
tend to be transmitted to both sexes, here wholly fails. But this rule, as
before remarked, does not appear to be nearly so general as the converse one,
namely, that characters which appear late in life in one sex are transmitted
exclusively to the same sex. From the fact of the above abnormal peculiarities
becoming attached to one sex, long before the sexual functions are active, we
may infer that there must be some difference between the sexes at an extremely
early age. With respect to sexually-limited diseases, we know too little of the
period at which they originate, to draw any safe conclusion. Gout, however,
seems to fall under our rule, for it is generally caused by intemperance during
manhood, and is transmitted from the father to his sons in a much more marked
manner than to his daughters.</p>
<p>In the various domestic breeds of sheep, goats, and cattle, the males differ
from their respective females in the shape or development of their horns,
forehead, mane, dewlap, tail, and hump on the shoulders; and these
peculiarities, in accordance with our rule, are not fully developed until a
rather late period of life. The sexes of dogs do not differ, except that in
certain breeds, especially in the Scotch deer-hound, the male is much larger
and heavier than the female; and, as we shall see in a future chapter, the male
goes on increasing in size to an unusually late period of life, which,
according to rule, will account for his increased size being transmitted to his
male offspring alone. On the other hand, the tortoise-shell colour, which is
confined to female cats, is quite distinct at birth, and this case violates the
rule. There is a breed of pigeons in which the males alone are streaked with
black, and the streaks can be detected even in the nestlings; but they become
more conspicuous at each successive moult, so that this case partly opposes and
partly supports the rule. With the English Carrier and Pouter pigeons, the full
development of the wattle and the crop occurs rather late in life, and
conformably with the rule, these characters are transmitted in full perfection
to the males alone. The following cases perhaps come within the class
previously alluded to, in which both sexes have varied in the same manner at a
rather late period of life, and have consequently transferred their new
characters to both sexes at a corresponding late period; and if so, these cases
are not opposed to our rule:—there exist sub-breeds of the pigeon,
described by Neumeister (46. ‘Das Ganze der Taubenzucht,’ 1837, ss.
21, 24. For the case of the streaked pigeons, see Dr. Chapuis, ‘Le pigeon
voyageur Belge,’ 1865, p. 87.), in which both sexes change their colour
during two or three moults (as is likewise the case with the Almond Tumbler);
nevertheless, these changes, though occurring rather late in life, are common
to both sexes. One variety of the Canary-bird, namely the London Prize, offers
a nearly analogous case.</p>
<p>With the breeds of the Fowl the inheritance of various characters by one or
both sexes, seems generally determined by the period at which such characters
are developed. Thus in all the many breeds in which the adult male differs
greatly in colour from the female, as well as from the wild parent-species, he
differs also from the young male, so that the newly-acquired characters must
have appeared at a rather late period of life. On the other hand, in most of
the breeds in which the two sexes resemble each other, the young are coloured
in nearly the same manner as their parents, and this renders it probable that
their colours first appeared early in life. We have instances of this fact in
all black and white breeds, in which the young and old of both sexes are alike;
nor can it be maintained that there is something peculiar in a black or white
plumage, which leads to its transference to both sexes; for the males alone of
many natural species are either black or white, the females being differently
coloured. With the so-called Cuckoo sub-breeds of the fowl, in which the
feathers are transversely pencilled with dark stripes, both sexes and the
chickens are coloured in nearly the same manner. The laced plumage of the
Sebright bantam is the same in both sexes, and in the young chickens the
wing-feathers are distinctly, though imperfectly laced. Spangled Hamburgs,
however, offer a partial exception; for the two sexes, though not quite alike,
resemble each other more closely than do the sexes of the aboriginal
parent-species; yet they acquire their characteristic plumage late in life, for
the chickens are distinctly pencilled. With respect to other characters besides
colour, in the wild-parent species and in most of the domestic breeds, the
males alone possess a well-developed comb; but in the young of the Spanish fowl
it is largely developed at a very early age, and, in accordance with this early
development in the male, it is of unusual size in the adult female. In the Game
breeds pugnacity is developed at a wonderfully early age, of which curious
proofs could be given; and this character is transmitted to both sexes, so that
the hens, from their extreme pugnacity, are now generally exhibited in separate
pens. With the Polish breeds the bony protuberance of the skull which supports
the crest is partially developed even before the chickens are hatched, and the
crest itself soon begins to grow, though at first feebly (47. For full
particulars and references on all these points respecting the several breeds of
the Fowl, see ‘Variation of Animals and Plants under
Domestication,’ vol. i. pp. 250, 256. In regard to the higher animals,
the sexual differences which have arisen under domestication are described in
the same work under the head of each species.); and in this breed the adults of
both sexes are characterised by a great bony protuberance and an immense crest.</p>
<p>Finally, from what we have now seen of the relation which exists in many
natural species and domesticated races, between the period of the development
of their characters and the manner of their transmission—for example, the
striking fact of the early growth of the horns in the reindeer, in which both
sexes bear horns, in comparison with their much later growth in the other
species in which the male alone bears horns—we may conclude that one,
though not the sole cause of characters being exclusively inherited by one sex,
is their development at a late age. And secondly, that one, though apparently a
less efficient cause of characters being inherited by both sexes, is their
development at an early age, whilst the sexes differ but little in
constitution. It appears, however, that some difference must exist between the
sexes even during a very early embryonic period, for characters developed at
this age not rarely become attached to one sex.</p>
<h3>A SUMMARY AND CONCLUDING REMARKS.</h3>
<p>From the foregoing discussion on the various laws of inheritance, we learn that
the characters of the parents often, or even generally, tend to become
developed in the offspring of the same sex, at the same age, and periodically
at the same season of the year, in which they first appeared in the parents.
But these rules, owing to unknown causes, are far from being fixed. Hence
during the modification of a species, the successive changes may readily be
transmitted in different ways; some to one sex, and some to both; some to the
offspring at one age, and some to the offspring at all ages. Not only are the
laws of inheritance extremely complex, but so are the causes which induce and
govern variability. The variations thus induced are preserved and accumulated
by sexual selection, which is in itself an extremely complex affair, depending,
as it does, on the ardour in love, the courage, and the rivalry of the males,
as well as on the powers of perception, the taste, and will of the female.
Sexual selection will also be largely dominated by natural selection tending
towards the general welfare of the species. Hence the manner in which the
individuals of either or both sexes have been affected through sexual selection
cannot fail to be complex in the highest degree.</p>
<p>When variations occur late in life in one sex, and are transmitted to the same
sex at the same age, the other sex and the young are left unmodified. When they
occur late in life, but are transmitted to both sexes at the same age, the
young alone are left unmodified. Variations, however, may occur at any period
of life in one sex or in both, and be transmitted to both sexes at all ages,
and then all the individuals of the species are similarly modified. In the
following chapters it will be seen that all these cases frequently occur in
nature.</p>
<p>Sexual selection can never act on any animal before the age for reproduction
arrives. From the great eagerness of the male it has generally acted on this
sex and not on the females. The males have thus become provided with weapons
for fighting with their rivals, with organs for discovering and securely
holding the female, and for exciting or charming her. When the sexes differ in
these respects, it is also, as we have seen, an extremely general law that the
adult male differs more or less from the young male; and we may conclude from
this fact that the successive variations, by which the adult male became
modified, did not generally occur much before the age for reproduction.
Whenever some or many of the variations occurred early in life, the young males
would partake more or less of the characters of the adult males; and
differences of this kind between the old and young males may be observed in
many species of animals.</p>
<p>It is probable that young male animals have often tended to vary in a manner
which would not only have been of no use to them at an early age, but would
have been actually injurious—as by acquiring bright colours, which would
render them conspicuous to their enemies, or by acquiring structures, such as
great horns, which would expend much vital force in their development.
Variations of this kind occurring in the young males would almost certainly be
eliminated through natural selection. With the adult and experienced males, on
the other hand, the advantages derived from the acquisition of such characters,
would more than counterbalance some exposure to danger, and some loss of vital
force.</p>
<p>As variations which give to the male a better chance of conquering other males,
or of finding, securing, or charming the opposite sex, would, if they happened
to arise in the female, be of no service to her, they would not be preserved in
her through sexual selection. We have also good evidence with domesticated
animals, that variations of all kinds are, if not carefully selected, soon lost
through intercrossing and accidental deaths. Consequently in a state of nature,
if variations of the above kind chanced to arise in the female line, and to be
transmitted exclusively in this line, they would be extremely liable to be
lost. If, however, the females varied and transmitted their newly acquired
characters to their offspring of both sexes, the characters which were
advantageous to the males would be preserved by them through sexual selection,
and the two sexes would in consequence be modified in the same manner, although
such characters were of no use to the females: but I shall hereafter have to
recur to these more intricate contingencies. Lastly, the females may acquire,
and apparently have often acquired by transference, characters from the male
sex.</p>
<p>As variations occurring later in life, and transmitted to one sex alone, have
incessantly been taken advantage of and accumulated through sexual selection in
relation to the reproduction of the species; therefore it appears, at first
sight, an unaccountable fact that similar variations have not frequently been
accumulated through natural selection, in relation to the ordinary habits of
life. If this had occurred, the two sexes would often have been differently
modified, for the sake, for instance, of capturing prey or of escaping from
danger. Differences of this kind between the two sexes do occasionally occur,
especially in the lower classes. But this implies that the two sexes follow
different habits in their struggles for existence, which is a rare circumstance
with the higher animals. The case, however, is widely different with the
reproductive functions, in which respect the sexes necessarily differ. For
variations in structure which are related to these functions, have often proved
of value to one sex, and from having arisen at a late period of life, have been
transmitted to one sex alone; and such variations, thus preserved and
transmitted, have given rise to secondary sexual characters.</p>
<p>In the following chapters, I shall treat of the secondary sexual characters in
animals of all classes, and shall endeavour in each case to apply the
principles explained in the present chapter. The lowest classes will detain us
for a very short time, but the higher animals, especially birds, must be
treated at considerable length. It should be borne in mind that for reasons
already assigned, I intend to give only a few illustrative instances of the
innumerable structures by the aid of which the male finds the female, or, when
found, holds her. On the other hand, all structures and instincts by the aid of
which the male conquers other males, and by which he allures or excites the
female, will be fully discussed, as these are in many ways the most
interesting.</p>
<div style="break-after:column;"></div><br />