<h2><SPAN name="link2HCH0015" id="link2HCH0015"></SPAN> CHAPTER XV.<br/> BIRDS—continued.</h2>
<p class="letter">
Discussion as to why the males alone of some species, and both sexes of others,
are brightly coloured—On sexually-limited inheritance, as applied to
various structures and to brightly-coloured plumage—Nidification in
relation to colour—Loss of nuptial plumage during the winter.</p>
<p>We have in this chapter to consider why the females of many birds have not
acquired the same ornaments as the male; and why, on the other hand, both sexes
of many other birds are equally, or almost equally, ornamented? In the
following chapter we shall consider the few cases in which the female is more
conspicuously coloured than the male.</p>
<p>In my ‘Origin of Species’ (1. Fourth edition, 1866, p. 241.) I
briefly suggested that the long tail of the peacock would be inconvenient and
the conspicuous black colour of the male capercailzie dangerous, to the female
during the period of incubation: and consequently that the transmission of
these characters from the male to the female offspring had been checked through
natural selection. I still think that this may have occurred in some few
instances: but after mature reflection on all the facts which I have been able
to collect, I am now inclined to believe that when the sexes differ, the
successive variations have generally been from the first limited in their
transmission to the same sex in which they first arose. Since my remarks
appeared, the subject of sexual coloration has been discussed in some very
interesting papers by Mr. Wallace (2. ‘Westminster Review,’ July
1867. ‘Journal of Travel,’ vol. i. 1868, p. 73.), who believes that
in almost all cases the successive variations tended at first to be transmitted
equally to both sexes; but that the female was saved, through natural
selection, from acquiring the conspicuous colours of the male, owing to the
danger which she would thus have incurred during incubation.</p>
<p>This view necessitates a tedious discussion on a difficult point, namely,
whether the transmission of a character, which is at first inherited by both
sexes can be subsequently limited in its transmission to one sex alone by means
of natural selection. We must bear in mind, as shewn in the preliminary chapter
on sexual selection, that characters which are limited in their development to
one sex are always latent in the other. An imaginary illustration will best aid
us in seeing the difficulty of the case; we may suppose that a fancier wished
to make a breed of pigeons, in which the males alone should be coloured of a
pale blue, whilst the females retained their former slaty tint. As with pigeons
characters of all kinds are usually transmitted to both sexes equally, the
fancier would have to try to convert this latter form of inheritance into
sexually-limited transmission. All that he could do would be to persevere in
selecting every male pigeon which was in the least degree of a paler blue; and
the natural result of this process, if steadily carried on for a long time, and
if the pale variations were strongly inherited or often recurred, would be to
make his whole stock of a lighter blue. But our fancier would be compelled to
match, generation after generation, his pale blue males with slaty females, for
he wishes to keep the latter of this colour. The result would generally be the
production either of a mongrel piebald lot, or more probably the speedy and
complete loss of the pale-blue tint; for the primordial slaty colour would be
transmitted with prepotent force. Supposing, however, that some pale-blue males
and slaty females were produced during each successive generation, and were
always crossed together, then the slaty females would have, if I may use the
expression, much blue blood in their veins, for their fathers, grandfathers,
etc., will all have been blue birds. Under these circumstances it is
conceivable (though I know of no distinct facts rendering it probable) that the
slaty females might acquire so strong a latent tendency to pale-blueness, that
they would not destroy this colour in their male offspring, their female
offspring still inheriting the slaty tint. If so, the desired end of making a
breed with the two sexes permanently different in colour might be gained.</p>
<p>The extreme importance, or rather necessity in the above case of the desired
character, namely, pale-blueness, being present though in a latent state in the
female, so that the male offspring should not be deteriorated, will be best
appreciated as follows: the male of Soemmerring’s pheasant has a tail
thirty-seven inches in length, whilst that of the female is only eight inches;
the tail of the male common pheasant is about twenty inches, and that of the
female twelve inches long. Now if the female Soemmerring pheasant with her
SHORT tail were crossed with the male common pheasant, there can be no doubt
that the male hybrid offspring would have a much LONGER tail than that of the
pure offspring of the common pheasant. On the other hand, if the female common
pheasant, with a tail much longer than that of the female Soemmerring pheasant,
were crossed with the male of the latter, the male hybrid offspring would have
a much SHORTER tail than that of the pure offspring of Soemmerring’s
pheasant. (3. Temminck says that the tail of the female Phasianus Soemmerringii
is only six inches long, ‘Planches coloriees,’ vol. v. 1838, pp.
487 and 488: the measurements above given were made for me by Mr. Sclater. For
the common pheasant, see Macgillivray, ‘History of British Birds,’
vol. i. pp. 118-121.)</p>
<p>Our fancier, in order to make his new breed with the males of a pale-blue tint,
and the females unchanged, would have to continue selecting the males during
many generations; and each stage of paleness would have to be fixed in the
males, and rendered latent in the females. The task would be an extremely
difficult one, and has never been tried, but might possibly be successfully
carried out. The chief obstacle would be the early and complete loss of the
pale-blue tint, from the necessity of reiterated crosses with the slaty female,
the latter not having at first any LATENT tendency to produce pale-blue
offspring.</p>
<p>On the other hand, if one or two males were to vary ever so slightly in
paleness, and the variations were from the first limited in their transmission
to the male sex, the task of making a new breed of the desired kind would be
easy, for such males would simply have to be selected and matched with ordinary
females. An analogous case has actually occurred, for there are breeds of the
pigeon in Belgium (4. Dr. Chapuis, ‘Le Pigeon Voyageur Belge,’
1865, p. 87.) in which the males alone are marked with black striae. So again
Mr. Tegetmeier has recently shewn (5. The ‘Field,’ Sept. 1872.)
that dragons not rarely produce silver-coloured birds, which are almost always
hens; and he himself has bred ten such females. It is on the other hand a very
unusual event when a silver male is produced; so that nothing would be easier,
if desired, than to make a breed of dragons with blue males and silver females.
This tendency is indeed so strong that when Mr. Tegetmeier at last got a silver
male and matched him with one of the silver females, he expected to get a breed
with both sexes thus coloured; he was however disappointed, for the young male
reverted to the blue colour of his grandfather, the young female alone being
silver. No doubt with patience this tendency to reversion in the males, reared
from an occasional silver male matched with a silver hen, might be eliminated,
and then both sexes would be coloured alike; and this very process has been
followed with success by Mr. Esquilant in the case of silver turbits.</p>
<p>With fowls, variations of colour, limited in their transmission to the male
sex, habitually occur. When this form of inheritance prevails, it might well
happen that some of the successive variations would be transferred to the
female, who would then slightly resemble the male, as actually occurs in some
breeds. Or again, the greater number, but not all, of the successive steps
might be transferred to both sexes, and the female would then closely resemble
the male. There can hardly be a doubt that this is the cause of the male pouter
pigeon having a somewhat larger crop, and of the male carrier pigeon having
somewhat larger wattles, than their respective females; for fanciers have not
selected one sex more than the other, and have had no wish that these
characters should be more strongly displayed in the male than in the female,
yet this is the case with both breeds.</p>
<p>The same process would have to be followed, and the same difficulties
encountered, if it were desired to make a breed with the females alone of some
new colour.</p>
<p>Lastly, our fancier might wish to make a breed with the two sexes differing
from each other, and both from the parent species. Here the difficulty would be
extreme, unless the successive variations were from the first sexually limited
on both sides, and then there would be no difficulty. We see this with the
fowl; thus the two sexes of the pencilled Hamburghs differ greatly from each
other, and from the two sexes of the aboriginal Gallus bankiva; and both are
now kept constant to their standard of excellence by continued selection, which
would be impossible unless the distinctive characters of both were limited in
their transmission.</p>
<p>The Spanish fowl offers a more curious case; the male has an immense comb, but
some of the successive variations, by the accumulation of which it was
acquired, appear to have been transferred to the female; for she has a comb
many times larger than that of the females of the parent species. But the comb
of the female differs in one respect from that of the male, for it is apt to
lop over; and within a recent period it has been ordered by the fancy that this
should always be the case, and success has quickly followed the order. Now the
lopping of the comb must be sexually limited in its transmission, otherwise it
would prevent the comb of the male from being perfectly upright, which would be
abhorrent to every fancier. On the other hand, the uprightness of the comb in
the male must likewise be a sexually-limited character, otherwise it would
prevent the comb of the female from lopping over.</p>
<p>From the foregoing illustrations, we see that even with almost unlimited time
at command, it would be an extremely difficult and complex, perhaps an
impossible process, to change one form of transmission into the other through
selection. Therefore, without distinct evidence in each case, I am unwilling to
admit that this has been effected in natural species. On the other hand, by
means of successive variations, which were from the first</p>
<p>sexually limited in their transmission, there would not be the least difficulty
in rendering a male bird widely different in colour or in any other character
from the female; the latter being left unaltered, or slightly altered, or
specially modified for the sake of protection.</p>
<p>As bright colours are of service to the males in their rivalry with other
males, such colours would be selected whether or not they were transmitted
exclusively to the same sex. Consequently the females might be expected often
to partake of the brightness of the males to a greater or less degree; and this
occurs with a host of species. If all the successive variations were
transmitted equally to both sexes, the females would be indistinguishable from
the males; and this likewise occurs with many birds. If, however, dull colours
were of high importance for the safety of the female during incubation, as with
many ground birds, the females which varied in brightness, or which received
through inheritance from the males any marked accession of brightness, would
sooner or later be destroyed. But the tendency in the males to continue for an
indefinite period transmitting to their female offspring their own brightness,
would have to be eliminated by a change in the form of inheritance; and this,
as shewn by our previous illustration, would be extremely difficult. The more
probable result of the long-continued destruction of the more brightly-coloured
females, supposing the equal form of transmission to prevail, would be the
lessening or annihilation of the bright colours of the males, owing to their
continual crossing with the duller females. It would be tedious to follow out
all the other possible results; but I may remind the reader that if
sexually-limited variations in brightness occurred in the females, even if they
were not in the least injurious to them and consequently were not eliminated,
yet they would not be favoured or selected, for the male usually accepts any
female, and does not select the more attractive individuals; consequently these
variations would be liable to be lost, and would have little influence on the
character of the race; and this will aid in accounting for the females being
commonly duller-coloured than the males.</p>
<p>In the eighth chapter instances were given, to which many might here be added,
of variations occurring at various ages, and inherited at the corresponding
age. It was also shewn that variations which occur late in life are commonly
transmitted to the same sex in which they first appear; whilst variations
occurring early in life are apt to be transmitted to both sexes; not that all
the cases of sexually-limited transmission can thus be accounted for. It was
further shewn that if a male bird varied by becoming brighter whilst young,
such variations would be of no service until the age for reproduction had
arrived, and there was competition between rival males. But in the case of
birds living on the ground and commonly in need of the protection of dull
colours, bright tints would be far more dangerous to the young and
inexperienced than to the adult males. Consequently the males which varied in
brightness whilst young would suffer much destruction and be eliminated through
natural selection; on the other hand, the males which varied in this manner
when nearly mature, notwithstanding that they were exposed to some additional
danger, might survive, and from being favoured through sexual selection, would
procreate their kind. As a relation often exists between the period of
variation and the form of transmission, if the bright-coloured young males were
destroyed and the mature ones were successful in their courtship, the males
alone would acquire brilliant colours and would transmit them exclusively to
their male offspring. But I by no means wish to maintain that the influence of
age on the form of transmission, is the sole cause of the great difference in
brilliancy between the sexes of many birds.</p>
<p>When the sexes of birds differ in colour, it is interesting to determine
whether the males alone have been modified by sexual selection, the females
having been left unchanged, or only partially and indirectly thus changed; or
whether the females have been specially modified through natural selection for
the sake of protection. I will therefore discuss this question at some length,
even more fully than its intrinsic importance deserves; for various curious
collateral points may thus be conveniently considered.</p>
<p>Before we enter on the subject of colour, more especially in reference to Mr.
Wallace’s conclusions, it may be useful to discuss some other sexual
differences under a similar point of view. A breed of fowls formerly existed in
Germany (6. Bechstein, ‘Naturgeschichte Deutschlands,’ 1793, B.
iii. 339.) in which the hens were furnished with spurs; they were good layers,
but they so greatly disturbed their nests with their spurs that they could not
be allowed to sit on their own eggs. Hence at one time it appeared to me
probable that with the females of the wild Gallinaceae the development of spurs
had been checked through natural selection, from the injury thus caused to
their nests. This seemed all the more probable, as wing-spurs, which would not
be injurious during incubation, are often as well-developed in the female as in
the male; though in not a few cases they are rather larger in the male. When
the male is furnished with leg-spurs the female almost always exhibits
rudiments of them,—the rudiment sometimes consisting of a mere scale, as
in Gallus. Hence it might be argued that the females had aboriginally been
furnished with well-developed spurs, but that these had subsequently been lost
through disuse or natural selection. But if this view be admitted, it would
have to be extended to innumerable other cases; and it implies that the female
progenitors of the existing spur-bearing species were once encumbered with an
injurious appendage.</p>
<p>In some few genera and species, as in Galloperdix, Acomus, and the Javan
peacock (Pavo muticus), the females, as well as the males, possess
well-developed leg-spurs. Are we to infer from this fact that they construct a
different sort of nest from that made by their nearest allies, and not liable
to be injured by their spurs; so that the spurs have not been removed? Or are
we to suppose that the females of these several species especially require
spurs for their defence? It is a more probable conclusion that both the
presence and absence of spurs in the females result from different laws of
inheritance having prevailed, independently of natural selection. With the many
females in which spurs appear as rudiments, we may conclude that some few of
the successive variations, through which they were developed in the males,
occurred very early in life, and were consequently transferred to the females.
In the other and much rarer cases, in which the females possess fully developed
spurs, we may conclude that all the successive variations were transferred to
them; and that they gradually acquired and inherited the habit of not
disturbing their nests.</p>
<p>The vocal organs and the feathers variously modified for producing sound, as
well as the proper instincts for using them, often differ in the two sexes, but
are sometimes the same in both. Can such differences be accounted for by the
males having acquired these organs and instincts, whilst the females have been
saved from inheriting them, on account of the danger to which they would have
been exposed by attracting the attention of birds or beasts of prey? This does
not seem to me probable, when we think of the multitude of birds which with
impunity gladden the country with their voices during the spring. (7. Daines
Barrington, however, thought it probable (‘Philosophical
Transactions,’ 1773, p. 164) that few female birds sing, because the
talent would have been dangerous to them during incubation. He adds, that a
similar view may possibly account for the inferiority of the female to the male
in plumage.) It is a safer conclusion that, as vocal and instrumental organs
are of special service only to the males during their courtship, these organs
were developed through sexual selection and their constant use in that sex
alone—the successive variations and the effects of use having been from
the first more or less limited in transmission to the male offspring.</p>
<p>Many analogous cases could be adduced; those for instance of the plumes on the
head being generally longer in the male than in the female, sometimes of equal
length in both sexes, and occasionally absent in the female,—these
several cases occurring in the same group of birds. It would be difficult to
account for such a difference between the sexes by the female having been
benefited by possessing a slightly shorter crest than the male, and its
consequent diminution or complete suppression through natural selection. But I
will take a more favourable case, namely the length of the tail. The long train
of the peacock would have been not only inconvenient but dangerous to the
peahen during the period of incubation and whilst accompanying her young. Hence
there is not the least a priori improbability in the development of her tail
having been checked through natural selection. But the females of various
pheasants, which apparently are exposed on their open nests to as much danger
as the peahen, have tails of considerable length. The females as well as the
males of the Menura superba have long tails, and they build a domed nest, which
is a great anomaly in so large a bird. Naturalists have wondered how the female
Menura could manage her tail during incubation; but it is now known (8. Mr.
Ramsay, in ‘Proc. Zoolog. Soc.’ 1868, p. 50.) that she
“enters the nest head first, and then turns round with her tail sometimes
over her back, but more often bent round by her side. Thus in time the tail
becomes quite askew, and is a tolerable guide to the length of time the bird
has been sitting.” Both sexes of an Australian kingfisher (Tanysiptera
sylvia) have the middle tail-feathers greatly lengthened, and the female makes
her nest in a hole; and as I am informed by Mr. R.B. Sharpe these feathers
become much crumpled during incubation.</p>
<p>In these two latter cases the great length of the tail-feathers must be in some
degree inconvenient to the female; and as in both species the tail-feathers of
the female are somewhat shorter than those of the male, it might be argued that
their full development had been prevented through natural selection. But if the
development of the tail of the peahen had been checked only when it became
inconveniently or dangerously great, she would have retained a much longer tail
than she actually possesses; for her tail is not nearly so long, relatively to
the size of her body, as that of many female pheasants, nor longer than that of
the female turkey. It must also be borne in mind that, in accordance with this
view, as soon as the tail of the peahen became dangerously long, and its
development was consequently checked, she would have continually reacted on her
male progeny, and thus have prevented the peacock from acquiring his present
magnificent train. We may therefore infer that the length of the tail in the
peacock and its shortness in the peahen are the result of the requisite
variations in the male having been from the first transmitted to the male
offspring alone.</p>
<p>We are led to a nearly similar conclusion with respect to the length of the
tail in the various species of pheasants. In the Eared pheasant (Crossoptilon
auritum) the tail is of equal length in both sexes, namely sixteen or seventeen
inches; in the common pheasant it is about twenty inches long in the male and
twelve in the female; in Soemmerring’s pheasant, thirty-seven inches in
the male and only eight in the female; and lastly in Reeve’s pheasant it
is sometimes actually seventy-two inches long in the male and sixteen in the
female. Thus in the several species, the tail of the female differs much in
length, irrespectively of that of the male; and this can be accounted for, as
it seems to me, with much more probability, by the laws of
inheritance,—that is by the successive variations having been from the
first more or less closely limited in their transmission to the male sex than
by the agency of natural selection, resulting from the length of tail being
more or less injurious to the females of these several allied species.</p>
<div style="break-after:column;"></div><br />