<p>Male quadrupeds, which are furnished with tusks, use them in various ways, as
in the case of horns. The boar strikes laterally and upwards; the musk-deer
downwards with serious effect. (28. Pallas, ‘Spicilegia Zoologica,’
fasc. xiii. 1779, p. 18.) The walrus, though having so short a neck and so
unwieldy a body, “can strike either upwards, or downwards, or sideways,
with equal dexterity.” (29. Lamont, ‘Seasons with the
Sea-Horses,’ 1861, p. 141.) I was informed by the late Dr. Falconer, that
the Indian elephant fights in a different manner according to the position and
curvature of his tusks. When they are directed forwards and upwards he is able
to fling a tiger to a great distance—it is said to even thirty feet; when
they are short and turned downwards he endeavours suddenly to pin the tiger to
the ground and, in consequence, is dangerous to the rider, who is liable to be
jerked off the howdah. (30. See also Corse (‘Philosophical
Transactions,’ 1799, p. 212) on the manner in which the short-tusked
Mooknah variety attacks other elephants.)</p>
<p>Very few male quadrupeds possess weapons of two distinct kinds specially
adapted for fighting with rival males. The male muntjac-deer (Cervulus),
however, offers an exception, as he is provided with horns and exserted canine
teeth. But we may infer from what follows that one form of weapon has often
been replaced in the course of ages by another. With ruminants the development
of horns generally stands in an inverse relation with that of even moderately
developed canine teeth. Thus camels, guanacoes, chevrotains, and musk-deer, are
hornless, and they have efficient canines; these teeth being “always of
smaller size in the females than in the males.” The Camelidae have, in
addition to their true canines, a pair of canine-shaped incisors in their upper
jaws. (31. Owen, ‘Anatomy of Vertebrates,’ vol. iii. p. 349.) Male
deer and antelopes, on the other hand, possess horns, and they rarely have
canine teeth; and these, when present, are always of small size, so that it is
doubtful whether they are of any service in their battles. In Antilope montana
they exist only as rudiments in the young male, disappearing as he grows old;
and they are absent in the female at all ages; but the females of certain other
antelopes and of certain deer have been known occasionally to exhibit rudiments
of these teeth. (32. See Ruppell (in ‘Proc. Zoolog. Soc.’ Jan. 12,
1836, p. 3) on the canines in deer and antelopes, with a note by Mr. Martin on
a female American deer. See also Falconer (‘Palaeont. Memoirs and
Notes,’ vol. i. 1868, p. 576) on canines in an adult female deer. In old
males of the musk-deer the canines (Pallas, ‘Spic. Zoolog.’ fasc.
xiii. 1779, p. 18) sometimes grow to the length of three inches, whilst in old
females a rudiment projects scarcely half an inch above the gums.) Stallions
have small canine teeth, which are either quite absent or rudimentary in the
mare; but they do not appear to be used in fighting, for stallions bite with
their incisors, and do not open their mouths wide like camels and guanacoes.
Whenever the adult male possesses canines, now inefficient, whilst the female
has either none or mere rudiments, we may conclude that the early male
progenitor of the species was provided with efficient canines, which have been
partially transferred to the females. The reduction of these teeth in the males
seems to have followed from some change in their manner of fighting, often (but
not in the horse) caused by the development of new weapons.</p>
<p>Tusks and horns are manifestly of high importance to their possessors, for
their development consumes much organised matter. A single tusk of the Asiatic
elephant—one of the extinct woolly species—and of the African
elephant, have been known to weigh respectively 150, 160, and 180 pounds; and
even greater weights have been given by some authors. (33. Emerson Tennent,
‘Ceylon,’ 1859, vol. ii. p. 275; Owen, ‘British Fossil
Mammals,’ 1846, p. 245.) With deer, in which the horns are periodically
renewed, the drain on the constitution must be greater; the horns, for
instance, of the moose weigh from fifty to sixty pounds, and those of the
extinct Irish elk from sixty to seventy pounds—the skull of the latter
weighing on an average only five pounds and a quarter. Although the horns are
not periodically renewed in sheep, yet their development, in the opinion of
many agriculturists, entails a sensible loss to the breeder. Stags, moreover,
in escaping from beasts of prey are loaded with an additional weight for the
race, and are greatly retarded in passing through a woody country. The moose,
for instance, with horns extending five and a half feet from tip to tip,
although so skilful in their use that he will not touch or break a twig when
walking quietly, cannot act so dexterously whilst rushing away from a pack of
wolves. “During his progress he holds his nose up, so as to lay the horns
horizontally back; and in this attitude cannot see the ground
distinctly.” (34. Richardson, ‘Fauna Bor. Americana,’ on the
moose, Alces palmata, pp. 236, 237; on the expanse of the horns, ‘Land
and Water,’ 1869, p. 143. See also Owen, ‘British Fossil
Mammals,’ on the Irish elk, pp. 447, 455.) The tips of the horns of the
great Irish elk were actually eight feet apart! Whilst the horns are covered
with velvet, which lasts with red-deer for about twelve weeks, they are
extremely sensitive to a blow; so that in Germany the stags at this time
somewhat change their habits, and avoiding dense forests, frequent young woods
and low thickets. (35. ‘Forest Creatures,’ by C. Boner, 1861, p.
60.) These facts remind us that male birds have acquired ornamental plumes at
the cost of retarded flight, and other ornaments at the cost of some loss of
power in their battles with rival males.</p>
<p>With mammals, when, as is often the case, the sexes differ in size, the males
are almost always larger and stronger. I am informed by Mr. Gould that this
holds good in a marked manner with the marsupials of Australia, the males of
which appear to continue growing until an unusually late age. But the most
extraordinary case is that of one of the seals (Callorhinus ursinus), a
full-grown female weighing less than one-sixth of a full-grown male. (36. See
the very interesting paper by Mr. J.A. Allen in ‘Bull. Mus. Comp. Zoology
of Cambridge, United States,’ vol. ii. No. 1, p. 82. The weights were
ascertained by a careful observer, Capt. Bryant. Dr. Gill in ‘The
American Naturalist,’ January, 1871, Prof. Shaler on the relative size of
the sexes of whales, ‘American Naturalist,’ January, 1873.) Dr.
Gill remarks that it is with the polygamous seals, the males of which are well
known to fight savagely together, that the sexes differ much in size; the
monogamous species differing but little. Whales also afford evidence of the
relation existing between the pugnacity of the males and their large size
compared with that of the female; the males of the right-whales do not fight
together, and they are not larger, but rather smaller, than their females; on
the other hand, male sperm-whales fight much together, and their bodies are
“often found scarred with the imprint of their rival’s
teeth,” and they are double the size of the females. The greater strength
of the male, as Hunter long ago remarked (37. ‘Animal Economy,’ p.
45.), is invariably displayed in those parts of the body which are brought into
action in fighting with rival males—for instance, in the massive neck of
the bull. Male quadrupeds are also more courageous and pugnacious than the
females. There can be little doubt that these characters have been gained,
partly through sexual selection, owing to a long series of victories, by the
stronger and more courageous males over the weaker, and partly through the
inherited effects of use. It is probable that the successive variations in
strength, size, and courage, whether due to mere variability or to the effects
of use, by the accumulation of which male quadrupeds have acquired these
characteristic qualities, occurred rather late in life, and were consequently
to a large extent limited in their transmission to the same sex.</p>
<p>From these considerations I was anxious to obtain information as to the Scotch
deer-hound, the sexes of which differ more in size than those of any other
breed (though blood-hounds differ considerably), or than in any wild canine
species known to me. Accordingly, I applied to Mr. Cupples, well known for his
success with this breed, who has weighed and measured many of his own dogs, and
who has with great kindness collected for me the following facts from various
sources. Fine male dogs, measured at the shoulder, range from 28 inches, which
is low, to 33 or even 34 inches in height; and in weight from 80 pounds, which
is light, to 120 pounds, or even more. The females range in height from 23 to
27, or even to 28 inches; and in weight from 50 to 70, or even 80 pounds. (38.
See also Richardson’s ‘Manual on the Dog,’ p. 59. Much
valuable information on the Scottish deer-hound is given by Mr. McNeill, who
first called attention to the inequality in size between the sexes, in
Scrope’s ‘Art of Deer-Stalking.’ I hope that Mr. Cupples will
keep to his intention of publishing a full account and history of this famous
breed.) Mr. Cupples concludes that from 95 to 100 pounds for the male, and 70
for the female, would be a safe average; but there is reason to believe that
formerly both sexes attained a greater weight. Mr. Cupples has weighed puppies
when a fortnight old; in one litter the average weight of four males exceeded
that of two females by six and a half ounces; in another litter the average
weight of four males exceeded that of one female by less than one ounce; the
same males when three weeks old, exceeded the female by seven and a half
ounces, and at the age of six weeks by nearly fourteen ounces. Mr. Wright of
Yeldersley House, in a letter to Mr. Cupples, says: “I have taken notes
on the sizes and weights of puppies of many litters, and as far as my
experience goes, dog-puppies as a rule differ very little from bitches till
they arrive at about five or six months old; and then the dogs begin to
increase, gaining upon the bitches both in weight and size. At birth, and for
several weeks afterwards, a bitch-puppy will occasionally be larger than any of
the dogs, but they are invariably beaten by them later.” Mr. McNeill, of
Colonsay, concludes that “the males do not attain their full growth till
over two years old, though the females attain it sooner.” According to
Mr. Cupples’ experience, male dogs go on growing in stature till they are
from twelve to eighteen months old, and in weight till from eighteen to
twenty-four months old; whilst the females cease increasing in stature at the
age of from nine to fourteen or fifteen months, and in weight at the age of
from twelve to fifteen months. From these various statements it is clear that
the full difference in size between the male and female Scotch deer-hound is
not acquired until rather late in life. The males almost exclusively are used
for coursing, for, as Mr. McNeill informs me, the females have not sufficient
strength and weight to pull down a full-grown deer. From the names used in old
legends, it appears, as I hear from Mr. Cupples, that, at a very ancient
period, the males were the most celebrated, the females being mentioned only as
the mothers of famous dogs. Hence, during many generations, it is the male
which has been chiefly tested for strength, size, speed, and courage, and the
best will have been bred from. As, however, the males do not attain their full
dimensions until rather late in life, they will have tended, in accordance with
the law often indicated, to transmit their characters to their male offspring
alone; and thus the great inequality in size between the sexes of the Scotch
deer-hound may probably be accounted for.</p>
<p>[Fig. 65. Head of Common wild boar, in prime of life (from Brehm).]</p>
<p>The males of some few quadrupeds possess organs or parts developed solely as a
means of defence against the attacks of other males. Some kinds of deer use, as
we have seen, the upper branches of their horns chiefly or exclusively for
defending themselves; and the Oryx antelope, as I am informed by Mr. Bartlett,
fences most skilfully with his long, gently curved horns; but these are
likewise used as organs of offence. The same observer remarks that rhinoceroses
in fighting, parry each other’s sidelong blows with their horns, which
clatter loudly together, as do the tusks of boars. Although wild boars fight
desperately, they seldom, according to Brehm, receive fatal wounds, as the
blows fall on each other’s tusks, or on the layer of gristly skin
covering the shoulder, called by the German hunters, the shield; and here we
have a part specially modified for defence. With boars in the prime of life
(Fig. 65) the tusks in the lower jaw are used for fighting, but they become in
old age, as Brehm states, so much curved inwards and upwards over the snout
that they can no longer be used in this way. They may, however, still serve,
and even more effectively, as a means of defence. In compensation for the loss
of the lower tusks as weapons of offence, those in the upper jaw, which always
project a little laterally, increase in old age so much in length and curve so
much upwards that they can be used for attack. Nevertheless, an old boar is not
so dangerous to man as one at the age of six or seven years. (39. Brehm,
‘Thierleben,’ B. ii. ss. 729-732.)</p>
<p>[Fig. 66. Skull of the Babirusa Pig (from Wallace’s ‘Malay
Archipelago’).]</p>
<p>In the full-grown male Babirusa pig of Celebes (Fig. 66), the lower tusks are
formidable weapons, like those of the European boar in the prime of life,
whilst the upper tusks are so long and have their points so much curled
inwards, sometimes even touching the forehead, that they are utterly useless as
weapons of attack. They more nearly resemble horns than teeth, and are so
manifestly useless as teeth that the animal was formerly supposed to rest his
head by hooking them on to a branch! Their convex surfaces, however, if the
head were held a little laterally, would serve as an excellent guard; and
hence, perhaps, it is that in old animals they “are generally broken off,
as if by fighting.” (40. See Mr. Wallace’s interesting account of
this animal, ‘The Malay Archipelago,’ 1869, vol. i. p. 435.) Here,
then, we have the curious case of the upper tusks of the Babirusa regularly
assuming during the prime of life a structure which apparently renders them
fitted only for defence; whilst in the European boar the lower tusks assume in
a less degree and only during old age nearly the same form, and then serve in
like manner solely for defence.</p>
<p>[Fig. 67. Head of female Aethiopian wart-hog, from ‘Proc. Zool.
Soc.’ 1869, shewing the same characters as the male, though on a reduced
scale. N.B. When the engraving was first made, I was under the impression that
it represented the male.]</p>
<p>In the wart-hog (see Phacochoerus aethiopicus, Fig. 67) the tusks in the upper
jaw of the male curve upwards during the prime of life, and from being pointed
serve as formidable weapons. The tusks in the lower jaw are sharper than those
in the upper, but from their shortness it seems hardly possible that they can
be used as weapons of attack. They must, however, greatly strengthen those in
the upper jaw, from being ground so as to fit closely against their bases.
Neither the upper nor the lower tusks appear to have been specially modified to
act as guards, though no doubt they are to a certain extent used for this
purpose. But the wart-hog is not destitute of other special means of
protection, for it has, on each side of the face, beneath the eyes, a rather
stiff, yet flexible, cartilaginous, oblong pad (Fig. 67), which projects two or
three inches outwards; and it appeared to Mr. Bartlett and myself, when viewing
the living animal, that these pads, when struck from beneath by the tusks of an
opponent, would be turned upwards, and would thus admirably protect the
somewhat prominent eyes. I may add, on the authority of Mr. Bartlett, that
these boars when fighting stand directly face to face.</p>
<p>Lastly, the African river-hog (Potomochoerus penicillatus) has a hard
cartilaginous knob on each side of the face beneath the eyes, which answers to
the flexible pad of the wart-hog; it has also two bony prominences on the upper
jaw above the nostrils. A boar of this species in the Zoological Gardens
recently broke into the cage of the wart-hog. They fought all night long, and
were found in the morning much exhausted, but not seriously wounded. It is a
significant fact, as shewing the purposes of the above-described projections
and excrescences, that these were covered with blood, and were scored and
abraded in an extraordinary manner.</p>
<p>Although the males of so many members of the pig family are provided with
weapons, and as we have just seen with means of defence, these weapons seem to
have been acquired within a rather late geological period. Dr. Forsyth Major
specifies (41. ‘Atti della Soc. Italiana di Sc. Nat.’ 1873, vol.
xv. fasc. iv.) several miocene species, in none of which do the tusks appear to
have been largely developed in the males; and Professor Rutimeyer was formerly
struck with this same fact.</p>
<p>The mane of the lion forms a good defence against the attacks of rival lions,
the one danger to which he is liable; for the males, as Sir A. Smith informs
me, engage in terrible battles, and a young lion dares not approach an old one.
In 1857 a tiger at Bromwich broke into the cage of a lion and a fearful scene
ensued: “the lion’s mane saved his neck and head from being much
injured, but the tiger at last succeeded in ripping up his belly, and in a few
minutes he was dead.” (42. ‘The Times,’ Nov. 10, 1857. In
regard to the Canada lynx, see Audubon and Bachman, ‘Quadrupeds of North
America,’ 1846, p. 139.) The broad ruff round the throat and chin of the
Canadian lynx (Felis canadensis) is much longer in the male than in the female;
but whether it serves as a defence I do not know. Male seals are well known to
fight desperately together, and the males of certain kinds (Otaria jubata) (43.
Dr. Murie, on Otaria, ‘Proc. Zoolog. Soc.’ 1869, p. 109. Mr. J.A.
Allen, in the paper above quoted (p. 75), doubts whether the hair, which is
longer on the neck in the male than in the female, deserves to be called a
mane.) have great manes, whilst the females have small ones or none. The male
baboon of the Cape of Good Hope (Cynocephalus porcarius) has a much longer mane
and larger canine teeth than the female; and the mane probably serves as a
protection, for, on asking the keepers in the Zoological Gardens, without
giving them any clue to my object, whether any of the monkeys especially
attacked each other by the nape of the neck, I was answered that this was not
the case, except with the above baboon. In the Hamadryas baboon, Ehrenberg
compares the mane of the adult male to that of a young lion, whilst in the
young of both sexes and in the female the mane is almost absent.</p>
<p>It appeared to me probable that the immense woolly mane of the male American
bison, which reaches almost to the ground, and is much more developed in the
males than in the females, served as a protection to them in their terrible
battles; but an experienced hunter told Judge Caton that he had never observed
anything which favoured this belief. The stallion has a thicker and fuller mane
than the mare; and I have made particular inquiries of two great trainers and
breeders, who have had charge of many entire horses, and am assured that they
“invariably endeavour to seize one another by the neck.” It does
not, however, follow from the foregoing statements, that when the hair on the
neck serves as a defence, that it was originally developed for this purpose,
though this is probable in some cases, as in that of the lion. I am informed by
Mr. McNeill that the long hairs on the throat of the stag (Cervus elaphus)
serve as a great protection to him when hunted, for the dogs generally
endeavour to seize him by the throat; but it is not probable that these hairs
were specially developed for this purpose; otherwise the young and the females
would have been equally protected.</p>
<h3>CHOICE IN PAIRING BY EITHER SEX OF QUADRUPEDS.</h3>
<p>Before describing in the next chapter, the differences between the sexes in
voice, odours emitted, and ornaments, it will be convenient here to consider
whether the sexes exert any choice in their unions. Does the female prefer any
particular male, either before or after the males may have fought together for
supremacy; or does the male, when not a polygamist, select any particular
female? The general impression amongst breeders seems to be that the male
accepts any female; and this owing to his eagerness, is, in most cases,
probably the truth. Whether the female as a general rule indifferently accepts
any male is much more doubtful. In the fourteenth chapter, on Birds, a
considerable body of direct and indirect evidence was advanced, shewing that
the female selects her partner; and it would be a strange anomaly if female
quadrupeds, which stand higher in the scale and have higher mental powers, did
not generally, or at least often, exert some choice. The female could in most
cases escape, if wooed by a male that did not please or excite her; and when
pursued by several males, as commonly occurs, she would often have the
opportunity, whilst they were fighting together, of escaping with some one
male, or at least of temporarily pairing with him. This latter contingency has
often been observed in Scotland with female red-deer, as I am informed by Sir
Philip Egerton and others. (44. Mr. Boner, in his excellent description of the
habits of the red-deer in Germany (‘Forest Creatures,’ 1861, p. 81)
says, “while the stag is defending his rights against one intruder,
another invades the sanctuary of his harem, and carries off trophy after
trophy.” Exactly the same thing occurs with seals; see Mr. J.A. Allen,
ibid. p. 100.)</p>
<p>It is scarcely possible that much should be known about female quadrupeds in a
state of nature making any choice in their marriage unions. The following
curious details on the courtship of one of the eared seals (Callorhinus
ursinus) are given (45. Mr. J.A. Allen in ‘Bull. Mus. Comp. Zoolog. of
Cambridge, United States,’ vol. ii. No. 1, p. 99.) on the authority of
Capt. Bryant, who had ample opportunities for observation. He says, “Many
of the females on their arrival at the island where they breed appear desirous
of returning to some particular male, and frequently climb the outlying rocks
to overlook the rookeries, calling out and listening as if for a familiar
voice. Then changing to another place they do the same again...As soon as a
female reaches the shore, the nearest male goes down to meet her, making
meanwhile a noise like the clucking of a hen to her chickens. He bows to her
and coaxes her until he gets between her and the water so that she cannot
escape him. Then his manner changes, and with a harsh growl he drives her to a
place in his harem. This continues until the lower row of harems is nearly
full. Then the males higher up select the time when their more fortunate
neighbours are off their guard to steal their wives. This they do by taking
them in their mouths and lifting them over the heads of the other females, and
carefully placing them in their own harem, carrying them as cats do their
kittens. Those still higher up pursue the same method until the whole space is
occupied. Frequently a struggle ensues between two males for the possession of
the same female, and both seizing her at once pull her in two or terribly
lacerate her with their teeth. When the space is all filled, the old male walks
around complacently reviewing his family, scolding those who crowd or disturb
the others, and fiercely driving off all intruders. This surveillance always
keeps him actively occupied.”</p>
<p>As so little is known about the courtship of animals in a state of nature, I
have endeavoured to discover how far our domesticated quadrupeds evince any
choice in their unions. Dogs offer the best opportunity for observation, as
they are carefully attended to and well understood. Many breeders have
expressed a strong opinion on this head. Thus, Mr. Mayhew remarks, “The
females are able to bestow their affections; and tender recollections are as
potent over them as they are known to be in other cases, where higher animals
are concerned. Bitches are not always prudent in their loves, but are apt to
fling themselves away on curs of low degree. If reared with a companion of
vulgar appearance, there often springs up between the pair a devotion which no
time can afterwards subdue. The passion, for such it really is, becomes of a
more than romantic endurance.” Mr. Mayhew, who attended chiefly to the
smaller breeds, is convinced that the females are strongly attracted by males
of a large size. (46. ‘Dogs: their Management,’ by E. Mayhew,
M.R.C.V.S., 2nd ed., 1864, pp. 187-192.) The well-known veterinary Blaine
states (47. Quoted by Alex. Walker, ‘On Intermarriage,’ 1838, p.
276; see also p. 244.) that his own female pug dog became so attached to a
spaniel, and a female setter to a cur, that in neither case would they pair
with a dog of their own breed until several weeks had elapsed. Two similar and
trustworthy accounts have been given me in regard to a female retriever and a
spaniel, both of which became enamoured with terrier-dogs.</p>
<p>Mr. Cupples informs me that he can personally vouch for the accuracy of the
following more remarkable case, in which a valuable and wonderfully-intelligent
female terrier loved a retriever belonging to a neighbour to such a degree,
that she had often to be dragged away from him. After their permanent
separation, although repeatedly shewing milk in her teats, she would never
acknowledge the courtship of any other dog, and to the regret of her owner
never bore puppies. Mr. Cupples also states, that in 1868, a female deerhound
in his kennel thrice produced puppies, and on each occasion shewed a marked
preference for one of the largest and handsomest, but not the most eager, of
four deerhounds living with her, all in the prime of life. Mr. Cupples has
observed that the female generally favours a dog whom she has associated with
and knows; her shyness and timidity at first incline her against a strange dog.
The male, on the contrary, seems rather inclined towards strange females. It
appears to be rare when the male refuses any particular female, but Mr. Wright,
of Yeldersley House, a great breeder of dogs, informs me that he has known some
instances; he cites the case of one of his own deerhounds, who would not take
any notice of a particular female mastiff, so that another deerhound had to be
employed. It would be superfluous to give, as I could, other instances, and I
will only add that Mr. Barr, who has carefully bred many bloodhounds, states
that in almost every instance particular individuals of opposite sexes shew a
decided preference for each other. Finally, Mr. Cupples, after attending to
this subject for another year, has written to me, “I have had full
confirmation of my former statement, that dogs in breeding form decided
preferences for each other, being often influenced by size, bright colour, and
individual characters, as well as by the degree of their previous
familiarity.”</p>
<p>In regard to horses, Mr. Blenkiron, the greatest breeder of race-horses in the
world, informs me that stallions are so frequently capricious in their choice,
rejecting one mare and without any apparent cause taking to another, that
various artifices have to be habitually used. The famous Monarque, for
instance, would never consciously look at the dam of Gladiateur, and a trick
had to be practised. We can partly see the reason why valuable race-horse
stallions, which are in such demand as to be exhausted, should be so particular
in their choice. Mr. Blenkiron has never known a mare reject a horse; but this
has occurred in Mr. Wright’s stable, so that the mare had to be cheated.
Prosper Lucas (48. ‘Traité de l’Héréd. Nat.’ tom. ii. 1850,
p. 296.) quotes various statements from French authorities, and remarks,
“On voit des étalons qui s’eprennent d’une jument, et
negligent toutes les autres.” He gives, on the authority of Baelen,
similar facts in regard to bulls; and Mr. H. Reeks assures me that a famous
short-horn bull belonging to his father “invariably refused to be matched
with a black cow.” Hoffberg, in describing the domesticated reindeer of
Lapland says, “Foeminae majores et fortiores mares prae caeteris
admittunt, ad eos confugiunt, a junioribus agitatae, qui hos in fugam
conjiciunt.” (49. ‘Amoenitates Acad.’ vol. iv. 1788, p. 160.)
A clergyman, who has bred many pigs, asserts that sows often reject one boar
and immediately accept another.</p>
<p>From these facts there can be no doubt that, with most of our domesticated
quadrupeds, strong individual antipathies and preferences are frequently
exhibited, and much more commonly by the female than by the male. This being
the case, it is improbable that the unions of quadrupeds in a state of nature
should be left to mere chance. It is much more probable that the females are
allured or excited by particular males, who possess certain characters in a
higher degree than other males; but what these characters are, we can seldom or
never discover with certainty.</p>
<!--end chapter-->
<h2><SPAN name="link2HCH0018" id="link2HCH0018"></SPAN> CHAPTER XVIII.<br/> SECONDARY SEXUAL CHARACTERS OF MAMMALS—continued.</h2>
<p class="letter">
Voice—Remarkable sexual peculiarities in
seals—Odour—Development of the hair—Colour of the hair and
skin—Anomalous case of the female being more ornamented than the
male—Colour and ornaments due to sexual selection—Colour acquired
for the sake of protection—Colour, though common to both sexes, often due
to sexual selection—On the disappearance of spots and stripes in adult
quadrupeds—On the colours and ornaments of the Quadrumana—Summary.</p>
<p>Quadrupeds use their voices for various purposes, as a signal of danger, as a
call from one member of a troop to another, or from the mother to her lost
offspring, or from the latter for protection to their mother; but such uses
need not here be considered. We are concerned only with the difference between
the voices of the sexes, for instance between that of the lion and lioness, or
of the bull and cow. Almost all male animals use their voices much more during
the rutting-season than at any other time; and some, as the giraffe and
porcupine (1. Owen, ‘Anatomy of Vertebrates,’ vol. iii. p. 585.),
are said to be completely mute excepting at this season. As the throats (i.e.
the larynx and thyroid bodies (2. Ibid. p. 595.)) of stags periodically become
enlarged at the beginning of the breeding-season, it might be thought that
their powerful voices must be somehow of high importance to them; but this is
very doubtful. From information given to me by two experienced observers, Mr.
McNeill and Sir P. Egerton, it seems that young stags under three years old do
not roar or bellow; and that the old ones begin bellowing at the commencement
of the breeding-season, at first only occasionally and moderately, whilst they
restlessly wander about in search of the females. Their battles are prefaced by
loud and prolonged bellowing, but during the actual conflict they are silent.
Animals of all kinds which habitually use their voices utter various noises
under any strong emotion, as when enraged and preparing to fight; but this may
merely be the result of nervous excitement, which leads to the spasmodic
contraction of almost all the muscles of the body, as when a man grinds his
teeth and clenches his fists in rage or agony. No doubt stags challenge each
other to mortal combat by bellowing; but those with the more powerful voices,
unless at the same time the stronger, better-armed, and more courageous, would
not gain any advantage over their rivals.</p>
<p>It is possible that the roaring of the lion may be of some service to him by
striking terror into his adversary; for when enraged he likewise erects his
mane and thus instinctively tries to make himself appear as terrible as
possible. But it can hardly be supposed that the bellowing of the stag, even if
it be of service to him in this way, can have been important enough to have led
to the periodical enlargement of the throat. Some writers suggest that the
bellowing serves as a call to the female; but the experienced observers above
quoted inform me that female deer do not search for the male, though the males
search eagerly for the females, as indeed might be expected from what we know
of the habits of other male quadrupeds. The voice of the female, on the other
hand, quickly brings to her one or more stags (3. See, for instance, Major W.
Ross King (‘The Sportsman in Canada,’ 1866, pp. 53, 131) on the
habits of the moose and wild reindeer.), as is well known to the hunters who in
wild countries imitate her cry. If we could believe that the male had the power
to excite or allure the female by his voice, the periodical enlargement of his
vocal organs would be intelligible on the principle of sexual selection,
together with inheritance limited to the same sex and season; but we have no
evidence in favour of this view. As the case stands, the loud voice of the stag
during the breeding-season does not seem to be of any special service to him,
either during his courtship or battles, or in any other way. But may we not
believe that the frequent use of the voice, under the strong excitement of
love, jealousy, and rage, continued during many generations, may at last have
produced an inherited effect on the vocal organs of the stag, as well as of
other male animals? This appears to me, in our present state of knowledge, the
most probable view.</p>
<p>The voice of the adult male gorilla is tremendous, and he is furnished with a
laryngeal sack, as is the adult male orang. (4. Owen ‘Anatomy of
Vertebrates,’ vol. iii. p. 600.) The gibbons rank among the noisiest of
monkeys, and the Sumatra species (Hylobates syndactylus) is also furnished with
an air sack; but Mr. Blyth, who has had opportunities for observation, does not
believe that the male is noisier than the female. Hence, these latter monkeys
probably use their voices as a mutual call; and this is certainly the case with
some quadrupeds, for instance the beaver. (5. Mr. Green, in ‘Journal of
Linnean Society,’ vol. x. ‘Zoology,’ 1869, note 362.) Another
gibbon, the H. agilis, is remarkable, from having the power of giving a
complete and correct octave of musical notes (6. C.L. Martin, ‘General
Introduction to the Natural History of Mamm. Animals,’ 1841, p. 431.),
which we may reasonably suspect serves as a sexual charm; but I shall have to
recur to this subject in the next chapter. The vocal organs of the American
Mycetes caraya are one-third larger in the male than in the female, and are
wonderfully powerful. These monkeys in warm weather make the forests resound at
morning and evening with their overwhelming voices. The males begin the
dreadful concert, and often continue it during many hours, the females
sometimes joining in with their less powerful voices. An excellent observer,
Rengger (7. ‘Naturgeschichte der Säugethiere von Paraguay,’ 1830,
ss. 15, 21.), could not perceive that they were excited to begin by any special
cause; he thinks that, like many birds, they delight in their own music, and
try to excel each other. Whether most of the foregoing monkeys have acquired
their powerful voices in order to beat their rivals and charm the
females—or whether the vocal organs have been strengthened and enlarged
through the inherited effects of long-continued use without any particular good
being thus gained—I will not pretend to say; but the former view, at
least in the case of the Hylobates agilis, seems the most probable.</p>
<p>I may here mention two very curious sexual peculiarities occurring in seals,
because they have been supposed by some writers to affect the voice. The nose
of the male sea-elephant (Macrorhinus proboscideus) becomes greatly elongated
during the breeding-season, and can then be erected. In this state it is
sometimes a foot in length. The female is not thus provided at any period of
life. The male makes a wild, hoarse, gurgling noise, which is audible at a
great distance and is believed to be strengthened by the proboscis; the voice
of the female being different. Lesson compares the erection of the proboscis,
with the swelling of the wattles of male gallinaceous birds whilst courting the
females. In another allied kind of seal, the bladder-nose (Cystophora
cristata), the head is covered by a great hood or bladder. This is supported by
the septum of the nose, which is produced far backwards and rises into an
internal crest seven inches in height. The hood is clothed with short hair, and
is muscular; can be inflated until it more than equals the whole head in size!
The males when rutting, fight furiously on the ice, and their roaring “is
said to be sometimes so loud as to be heard four miles off.” When
attacked they likewise roar or bellow; and whenever irritated the bladder is
inflated and quivers. Some naturalists believe that the voice is thus
strengthened, but various other uses have been assigned to this extraordinary
structure. Mr. R. Brown thinks that it serves as a protection against accidents
of all kinds; but this is not probable, for, as I am assured by Mr. Lamont who
killed 600 of these animals, the hood is rudimentary in the females, and it is
not developed in the males during youth. (8. On the sea-elephant, see an
article by Lesson, in ‘Dict. Class. Hist. Nat.’ tom. xiii. p. 418.
For the Cystophora, or Stemmatopus, see Dr. Dekay, ‘Annals of Lyceum of
Nat. Hist.’ New York, vol. i. 1824, p. 94. Pennant has also collected
information from the sealers on this animal. The fullest account is given by
Mr. Brown, in ‘Proc. Zoolog. Soc.’ 1868, p. 435.)</p>
<h3>ODOUR.</h3>
<p>With some animals, as with the notorious skunk of America, the overwhelming
odour which they emit appears to serve exclusively as a defence. With
shrew-mice (Sorex) both sexes possess abdominal scent-glands, and there can be
little doubt, from the rejection of their bodies by birds and beasts of prey,
that the odour is protective; nevertheless, the glands become enlarged in the
males during the breeding-season. In many other quadrupeds the glands are of
the same size in both sexes (9. As with the castoreum of the beaver, see Mr.
L.H. Morgan’s most interesting work, ‘The American Beaver,’
1868, p. 300. Pallas (‘Spic. Zoolog.’ fasc. viii. 1779, p. 23) has
well discussed the odoriferous glands of mammals. Owen (‘Anat. of
Vertebrates,’ vol. iii. p. 634) also gives an account of these glands,
including those of the elephant, and (p. 763) those of shrew-mice. On bats, Mr.
Dobson in ‘Proceedings of the Zoological Society’ 1873, p. 241.),
but their uses are not known. In other species the glands are confined to the
males, or are more developed than in the females; and they almost always become
more active during the rutting-season. At this period the glands on the sides
of the face of the male elephant enlarge, and emit a secretion having a strong
musky odour. The males, and rarely the females, of many kinds of bats have
glands and protrudable sacks situated in various parts; and it is believed that
these are odoriferous.</p>
<p>The rank effluvium of the male goat is well known, and that of certain male
deer is wonderfully strong and persistent. On the banks of the Plata I
perceived the air tainted with the odour of the male Cervus campestris, at half
a mile to leeward of a herd; and a silk handkerchief, in which I carried home a
skin, though often used and washed, retained, when first unfolded, traces of
the odour for one year and seven months. This animal does not emit its strong
odour until more than a year old, and if castrated whilst young never emits it.
(10. Rengger, ‘Naturgeschichte der Säugethiere von Paraguay,’ 1830,
s. 355. This observer also gives some curious particulars in regard to the
odour.) Besides the general odour, permeating the whole body of certain
ruminants (for instance, Bos moschatus) in the breeding-season, many deer,
antelopes, sheep, and goats possess odoriferous glands in various situations,
more especially on their faces. The so-called tear-sacks, or suborbital pits,
come under this head. These glands secrete a semi-fluid fetid matter which is
sometimes so copious as to stain the whole face, as I have myself seen in an
antelope. They are “usually larger in the male than in the female, and
their development is checked by castration.” (11. Owen, ‘Anatomy of
Vertebrates,’ vol. iii. p. 632. See also Dr. Murie’s observations
on those glands in the ‘Proc. Zoolog. Soc.’ 1870, p. 340.
Desmarest, ‘On the Antilope subgutturosa, ‘Mammalogie,’ 1820,
p. 455.) According to Desmarest they are altogether absent in the female of
Antilope subgutturosa. Hence, there can be no doubt that they stand in close
relation with the reproductive functions. They are also sometimes present, and
sometimes absent, in nearly allied forms. In the adult male musk-deer (Moschus
moschiferus), a naked space round the tail is bedewed with an odoriferous
fluid, whilst in the adult female, and in the male until two years old, this
space is covered with hair and is not odoriferous. The proper musk-sack of this
deer is from its position necessarily confined to the male, and forms an
additional scent-organ. It is a singular fact that the matter secreted by this
latter gland, does not, according to Pallas, change in consistence, or increase
in quantity, during the rutting-season; nevertheless this naturalist admits
that its presence is in some way connected with the act of reproduction. He
gives, however, only a conjectural and unsatisfactory explanation of its use.
(12. Pallas, ‘Spicilegia Zoolog.’ fasc. xiii. 1799, p. 24;
Desmoulins, ‘Dict. Class. d’Hist. Nat.’ tom. iii. p. 586.)</p>
<p>In most cases, when only the male emits a strong odour during the
breeding-season, it probably serves to excite or allure the female. We must not
judge on this head by our own taste, for it is well known that rats are enticed
by certain essential oils, and cats by valerian, substances far from agreeable
to us; and that dogs, though they will not eat carrion, sniff and roll on it.
From the reasons given when discussing the voice of the stag, we may reject the
idea that the odour serves to bring the females from a distance to the males.
Active and long-continued use cannot here have come into play, as in the case
of the vocal organs. The odour emitted must be of considerable importance to
the male, inasmuch as large and complex glands, furnished with muscles for
everting the sack, and for closing or opening the orifice, have in some cases
been developed. The development of these organs is intelligible through sexual
selection, if the most odoriferous males are the most successful in winning the
females, and in leaving offspring to inherit their gradually perfected glands
and odours.</p>
<h3>DEVELOPMENT OF THE HAIR.</h3>
<p>We have seen that male quadrupeds often have the hair on their necks and
shoulders much more developed than the females; and many additional instances
could be given. This sometimes serves as a defence to the male during his
battles; but whether the hair in most cases has been specially developed for
this purpose, is very doubtful. We may feel almost certain that this is not the
case, when only a thin and narrow crest runs along the back; for a crest of
this kind would afford scarcely any protection, and the ridge of the back is
not a place likely to be injured; nevertheless such crests are sometimes
confined to the males, or are much more developed in them than in the females.
Two antelopes, the Tragelaphus scriptus (13. Dr. Gray, ‘Gleanings from
the Menagerie at Knowsley,’ pl. 28.) (Fig. 70) and Portax picta may be
given as instances. When stags, and the males of the wild goat, are enraged or
terrified, these crests stand erect (14. Judge Caton on the Wapiti,
‘Transact. Ottawa Acad. Nat. Sciences,’ 1868, pp. 36, 40; Blyth,
‘Land and Water,’ on Capra aegagrus 1867, p. 37.); but it cannot be
supposed that they have been developed merely for the sake of exciting fear in
their enemies. One of the above-named antelopes, the Portax picta, has a large
well-defined brush of black hair on the throat, and this is much larger in the
male than in the female. In the Ammotragus tragelaphus of North Africa, a
member of the sheep-family, the fore-legs are almost concealed by an
extraordinary growth of hair, which depends from the neck and upper halves of
the legs; but Mr. Bartlett does not believe that this mantle is of the least
use to the male, in whom it is much more developed than in the female.</p>
<p>[Fig. 68. Pithecia satanas, male (from Brehm).]</p>
<p>Male quadrupeds of many kinds differ from the females in having more hair, or
hair of a different character, on certain parts of their faces. Thus the bull
alone has curled hair on the forehead. (15. Hunter’s ‘Essays and
Observations,’ edited by Owen, 1861. vol. i. p. 236.) In three
closely-allied sub-genera of the goat family, only the males possess beards,
sometimes of large size; in two other sub-genera both sexes have a beard, but
it disappears in some of the domestic breeds of the common goat; and neither
sex of the Hemitragus has a beard. In the ibex the beard is not developed
during the summer, and is so small at other times that it may be called
rudimentary. (16. See Dr. Gray’s ‘Catalogue of Mammalia in the
British Museum,’ part iii. 1852, p. 144.) With some monkeys the beard is
confined to the male, as in the orang; or is much larger in the male than in
the female, as in the Mycetes caraya and Pithecia satanas (Fig. 68). So it is
with the whiskers of some species of Macacus (17. Rengger,
‘Säugethiere,’ etc., s. 14; Desmarest, ‘Mammalogie,’ p.
86.), and, as we have seen, with the manes of some species of baboons. But with
most kinds of monkeys the various tufts of hair about the face and head are
alike in both sexes.</p>
<p>The males of various members of the ox family (Bovidae), and of certain
antelopes, are furnished with a dewlap, or great fold of skin on the neck,
which is much less developed in the female.</p>
<p>Now, what must we conclude with respect to such sexual differences as these? No
one will pretend that the beards of certain male goats, or the dewlaps of the
bull, or the crests of hair along the backs of certain male antelopes, are of
any use to them in their ordinary habits. It is possible that the immense beard
of the male Pithecia, and the large beard of the male orang, may protect their
throats when fighting; for the keepers in the Zoological Gardens inform me that
many monkeys attack each other by the throat; but it is not probable that the
beard has been developed for a distinct purpose from that served by the
whiskers, moustache, and other tufts of hair on the face; and no one will
suppose that these are useful as a protection. Must we attribute all these
appendages of hair or skin to mere purposeless variability in the male? It
cannot be denied that this is possible; for in many domesticated quadrupeds,
certain characters, apparently not derived through reversion from any wild
parent form, are confined to the males, or are more developed in them than in
the females—for instance, the hump on the male zebu-cattle of India, the
tail of fat-tailed rams, the arched outline of the forehead in the males of
several breeds of sheep, and lastly, the mane, the long hairs on the hind legs,
and the dewlap of the male of the Berbura goat. (18. See the chapters on these
several animals in vol. i. of my ‘Variation of Animals under
Domestication;’ also vol. ii. p. 73; also chap. xx. on the practice of
selection by semi-civilised people. For the Berbura goat, see Dr. Gray,
‘Catalogue,’ ibid. p. 157.) The mane, which occurs only in the rams
of an African breed of sheep, is a true secondary sexual character, for, as I
hear from Mr. Winwood Reade, it is not developed if the animal be castrated.
Although we ought to be extremely cautious, as shewn in my work on
‘Variation under Domestication,’ in concluding that any character,
even with animals kept by semi-civilised people, has not been subjected to
selection by man, and thus augmented, yet in the cases just specified this is
improbable; more especially as the characters are confined to the males, or are
more strongly developed in them than in the females. If it were positively
known that the above African ram is a descendant of the same primitive stock as
the other breeds of sheep, and if the Berbura male-goat with his mane, dewlap,
etc., is descended from the same stock as other goats, then, assuming that
selection has not been applied to these characters, they must be due to simple
variability, together with sexually-limited inheritance.</p>
<p>Hence it appears reasonable to extend this same view to all analogous cases
with animals in a state of nature. Nevertheless I cannot persuade myself that
it generally holds good, as in the case of the extraordinary development of
hair on the throat and fore-legs of the male Ammotragus, or in that of the
immense beard of the male Pithecia. Such study as I have been able to give to
nature makes me believe that parts or organs which are highly developed, were
acquired at some period for a special purpose. With those antelopes in which
the adult male is more strongly-coloured than the female, and with those
monkeys in which the hair on the face is elegantly arranged and coloured in a
diversified manner, it seems probable that the crests and tufts of hair were
gained as ornaments; and this I know is the opinion of some naturalists. If
this be correct, there can be little doubt that they were gained or at least
modified through sexual selection; but how far the same view may be extended to
other mammals is doubtful.</p>
<h3>COLOUR OF THE HAIR AND OF THE NAKED SKIN.</h3>
<p>I will first give briefly all the cases known to me of male quadrupeds
differing in colour from the females. With Marsupials, as I am informed by Mr.
Gould, the sexes rarely differ in this respect; but the great red kangaroo
offers a striking exception, “delicate blue being the prevailing tint in
those parts of the female which in the male are red.” (19. Osphranter
rufus, Gould, ‘Mammals of Australia,’ 1863, vol. ii. On the
Didelphis, Desmarest, ‘Mammalogie,’ p. 256.) In the Didelphis
opossum of Cayenne the female is said to be a little more red than the male. Of
the Rodents, Dr. Gray remarks: “African squirrels, especially those found
in the tropical regions, have the fur much brighter and more vivid at some
seasons of the year than at others, and the fur of the male is generally
brighter than that of the female.” (20. ‘Annals and Magazine of
Natural History,’ Nov. 1867, p. 325. On the Mus minutus, Desmarest,
‘Mammalogie,’ p. 304.) Dr. Gray informs me that he specified the
African squirrels, because, from their unusually bright colours, they best
exhibit this difference. The female of the Mus minutus of Russia is of a paler
and dirtier tint than the male. In a large number of bats the fur of the male
is lighter than in the female. (21. J.A. Allen, in ‘Bulletin of Mus.
Comp. Zoolog. of Cambridge, United States,’ 1869, p. 207. Mr. Dobson on
sexual characters in the Chiroptera, ‘Proceedings of the Zoological
Society,’ 1873, p. 241. Dr. Gray on Sloths, ibid. 1871, p. 436.) Mr.
Dobson also remarks, with respect to these animals: “Differences,
depending partly or entirely on the possession by the male of fur of a much
more brilliant hue, or distinguished by different markings or by the greater
length of certain portions, are met only, to any appreciable extent, in the
frugivorous bats in which the sense of sight is well developed.” This
last remark deserves attention, as bearing on the question whether bright
colours are serviceable to male animals from being ornamental. In one genus of
sloths, it is now established, as Dr. Gray states, “that the males are
ornamented differently from the females—that is to say, that they have a
patch of soft short hair between the shoulders, which is generally of a more or
less orange colour, and in one species pure white. The females, on the
contrary, are destitute of this mark.”</p>
<p>The terrestrial Carnivora and Insectivora rarely exhibit sexual differences of
any kind, including colour. The ocelot (Felis pardalis), however, is
exceptional, for the colours of the female, compared with those of the male,
are “moins apparentes, le fauve, étant plus terne, le blanc moins pur,
les raies ayant moins de largeur et les taches moins de diamètre.” (22.
Desmarest, ‘Mammalogie,’ 1820, p. 220. On Felis mitis, Rengger,
ibid. s. 194.) The sexes of the allied Felis mitis also differ, but in a less
degree; the general hues of the female being rather paler than in the male,
with the spots less black. The marine Carnivora or seals, on the other hand,
sometimes differ considerably in colour, and they present, as we have already
seen, other remarkable sexual differences. Thus the male of the Otaria
nigrescens of the southern hemisphere is of a rich brown shade above; whilst
the female, who acquires her adult tints earlier in life than the male, is
dark-grey above, the young of both sexes being of a deep chocolate colour. The
male of the northern Phoca groenlandica is tawny grey, with a curious
saddle-shaped dark mark on the back; the female is much smaller, and has a very
different appearance, being “dull white or yellowish straw-colour, with a
tawny hue on the back”; the young at first are pure white, and can
“hardly be distinguished among the icy hummocks and snow, their colour
thus acting as a protection.” (23. Dr. Murie on the Otaria,
‘Proceedings Zoological Society,’ 1869, p. 108. Mr. R. Brown on the
P. groenlandica, ibid. 1868, p. 417. See also on the colours of seals,
Desmarest, ibid. pp. 243, 249.)</p>
<p>With Ruminants sexual differences of colour occur more commonly than in any
other order. A difference of this kind is general in the Strepsicerene
antelopes; thus the male nilghau (Portax picta) is bluish-grey and much darker
than the female, with the square white patch on the throat, the white marks on
the fetlocks, and the black spots on the ears all much more distinct. We have
seen that in this species the crests and tufts of hair are likewise more
developed in the male than in the hornless female. I am informed by Mr. Blyth
that the male, without shedding his hair, periodically becomes darker during
the breeding-season. Young males cannot be distinguished from young females
until about twelve months old; and if the male is emasculated before this
period, he never, according to the same authority, changes colour. The
importance of this latter fact, as evidence that the colouring of the Portax is
of sexual origin, becomes obvious, when we hear (24. Judge Caton, in
‘Transactions of the Ottawa Academy of Natural Sciences,’ 1868, p.
4.) that neither the red summer-coat nor the blue winter-coat of the Virginian
deer is at all affected by emasculation. With most or all of the
highly-ornamented species of Tragelaphus the males are darker than the hornless
females, and their crests of hair are more fully developed. In the male of that
magnificent antelope, the Derbyan eland, the body is redder, the whole neck
much blacker, and the white band which separates these colours broader than in
the female. In the Cape eland, also, the male is slightly darker than the
female. (25. Dr. Gray, ‘Cat. of Mamm. in Brit. Mus.’ part iii.
1852, pp. 134-142; also Dr. Gray, ‘Gleanings from the Menagerie of
Knowsley,’ in which there is a splendid drawing of the Oreas derbianus:
see the text on Tragelaphus. For the Cape eland (Oreas canna), see Andrew
Smith, ‘Zoology of S. Africa,’ pl. 41 and 42. There are also many
of these Antelopes in the Zoological Gardens.)</p>
<p>In the Indian black-buck (A. bezoartica), which belongs to another tribe of
antelopes, the male is very dark, almost black; whilst the hornless female is
fawn-coloured. We meet in this species, as Mr. Blyth informs me, with an
exactly similar series of facts, as in the Portax picta, namely, in the male
periodically changing colour during the breeding-season, in the effects of
emasculation on this change, and in the young of both sexes being
indistinguishable from each other. In the Antilope niger the male is black, the
female, as well as the young of both sexes, being brown; in A. sing-sing the
male is much brighter coloured than the hornless female, and his chest and
belly are blacker; in the male A. caama, the marks and lines which occur on
various parts of the body are black, instead of brown as in the female; in the
brindled gnu (A. gorgon) “the colours of the male are nearly the same as
those of the female, only deeper and of a brighter hue.” (26. On the Ant.
niger, see ‘Proc. Zool. Soc.’ 1850, p. 133. With respect to an
allied species, in which there is an equal sexual difference in colour, see Sir
S. Baker, ‘The Albert Nyanza,’ 1866, vol. ii. p. 627. For the A.
sing-sing, Gray, ‘Cat. B. Mus.’ p. 100. Desmarest,
‘Mammalogie,’ p. 468, on the A. caama. Andrew Smith, ‘Zoology
of S. Africa,’ on the Gnu.) Other analogous cases could be added.</p>
<p>The Banteng bull (Bos sondaicus) of the Malayan Archipelago is almost black,
with white legs and buttocks; the cow is of a bright dun, as are the young
males until about the age of three years, when they rapidly change colour. The
emasculated bull reverts to the colour of the female. The female Kemas goat is
paler, and both it and the female Capra aegagrus are said to be more uniformly
tinted than their males. Deer rarely present any sexual differences in colour.
Judge Caton, however, informs me that in the males of the wapiti deer (Cervus
canadensis) the neck, belly, and legs are much darker than in the female; but
during the winter the darker tints gradually fade away and disappear. I may
here mention that Judge Caton has in his park three races of the Virginian
deer, which differ slightly in colour, but the differences are almost
exclusively confined to the blue winter or breeding-coat; so that this case may
be compared with those given in a previous chapter of closely-allied or
representative species of birds, which differ from each other only in their
breeding plumage. (27. ‘Ottawa Academy of Sciences,’ May 21, 1868,
pp. 3, 5.) The females of Cervus paludosus of S. America, as well as the young
of both sexes, do not possess the black stripes on the nose and the
blackish-brown line on the breast, which are characteristic of the adult males.
(28. S. Muller, on the Banteng, ‘Zoog. Indischen Archipel.’
1839-1844, tab. 35; see also Raffles, as quoted by Mr. Blyth, in ‘Land
and Water,’ 1867, p. 476. On goats, Dr. Gray, ‘Catalogue of the
British Museum,’ p. 146; Desmarest, ‘Mammalogie,’ p. 482. On
the Cervus paludosus, Rengger, ibid. s. 345.) Lastly, as I am informed by Mr.
Blyth, the mature male of the beautifully coloured and spotted axis deer is
considerably darker than the female: and this hue the castrated male never
acquires.</p>
<p>The last Order which we need consider is that of the Primates. The male of the
Lemur macaco is generally coal-black, whilst the female is brown. (29. Sclater,
‘Proc. Zool. Soc.’ 1866, p. i. The same fact has also been fully
ascertained by MM. Pollen and van Dam. See, also, Dr. Gray in ‘Annals and
Magazine of Natural History,’ May 1871, p. 340.) Of the Quadrumana of the
New World, the females and young of Mycetes caraya are greyish-yellow and like
each other; in the second year the young male becomes reddish-brown; in the
third, black, excepting the stomach, which, however, becomes quite black in the
fourth or fifth year. There is also a strongly-marked difference in colour
between the sexes of Mycetes seniculus and Cebus capucinus; the young of the
former, and I believe of the latter species, resembling the females. With
Pithecia leucocephala the young likewise resemble the females, which are
brownish-black above and light rusty-red beneath, the adult males being black.
The ruff of hair round the face of Ateles marginatus is tinted yellow in the
male and white in the female. Turning to the Old World, the males of Hylobates
hoolock are always black, with the exception of a white band over the brows;
the females vary from whity-brown to a dark tint mixed with black, but are
never wholly black. (30. On Mycetes, Rengger, ibid. s. 14; and Brehm,
‘Thierleben,’ B. i. s. 96, 107. On Ateles Desmarest,
‘Mammalogie,’ p. 75. On Hylobates, Blyth, ‘Land and
Water,’ 1867, p. 135. On the Semnopithecus, S. Muller, ‘Zoog.
Indischen Archipel.’ tab. x.) In the beautiful Cercopithecus diana, the
head of the adult male is of an intense black, whilst that of the female is
dark grey; in the former the fur between the thighs is of an elegant
fawn-colour, in the latter it is paler. In the beautiful and curious moustache
monkey (Cercopithecus cephus) the only difference between the sexes is that the
tail of the male is chestnut and that of the female grey; but Mr. Bartlett
informs me that all the hues become more pronounced in the male when adult,
whilst in the female they remain as they were during youth. According to the
coloured figures given by Solomon Muller, the male of Semnopithecus chrysomelas
is nearly black, the female being pale brown. In the Cercopithecus cynosurus
and griseo-viridis one part of the body, which is confined to the male sex, is
of the most brilliant blue or green, and contrasts strikingly with the naked
skin on the hinder part of the body, which is vivid red.</p>
<p>[Fig. 69. Head of male Mandrill (from Gervais, ‘Hist. Nat. des
Mammifères’).]</p>
<p>Lastly, in the baboon family, the adult male of Cynocephalus hamadryas differs
from the female not only by his immense mane, but slightly in the colour of the
hair and of the naked callosities. In the drill (C. leucophaeus) the females
and young are much paler-coloured, with less green, than the adult males. No
other member in the whole class of mammals is coloured in so extraordinary a
manner as the adult male mandrill (C. mormon). The face at this age becomes of
a fine blue, with the ridge and tip of the nose of the most brilliant red.
According to some authors, the face is also marked with whitish stripes, and is
shaded in parts with black, but the colours appear to be variable. On the
forehead there is a crest of hair, and on the chin a yellow beard.
“Toutes les parties supérieures de leurs cuisses et le grand espace nu de
leurs fesses sont également colorés du rouge le plus vif, avec un mélange de
bleu qui ne manque reellement pas d’élégance.” (31. Gervais,
‘Hist. Nat. des Mammifères,’ 1854, p. 103. Figures are given of the
skull of the male. Also Desmarest, ‘Mammalogie,’ p. 70. Geoffroy
St.-Hilaire and F. Cuvier, ‘Hist. Nat. des Mammifères,’ 1824, tom.
i.) When the animal is excited all the naked parts become much more vividly
tinted. Several authors have used the strongest expressions in describing these
resplendent colours, which they compare with those of the most brilliant birds.
Another remarkable peculiarity is that when the great canine teeth are fully
developed, immense protuberances of bone are formed on each cheek, which are
deeply furrowed longitudinally, and the naked skin over them is
brilliantly-coloured, as just-described. (Fig. 69.) In the adult females and in
the young of both sexes these protuberances are scarcely perceptible; and the
naked parts are much less bright coloured, the face being almost black, tinged
with blue. In the adult female, however, the nose at certain regular intervals
of time becomes tinted with red.</p>
<p>In all the cases hitherto given the male is more strongly or brighter coloured
than the female, and differs from the young of both sexes. But as with some few
birds it is the female which is brighter coloured than the male, so with the
Rhesus monkey (Macacus rhesus), the female has a large surface of naked skin
round the tail, of a brilliant carmine red, which, as I was assured by the
keepers in the Zoological Gardens, periodically becomes even yet more vivid,
and her face also is pale red. On the other hand, in the adult male and in the
young of both sexes (as I saw in the Gardens), neither the naked skin at the
posterior end of the body, nor the face, shew a trace of red. It appears,
however, from some published accounts, that the male does occasionally, or
during certain seasons, exhibit some traces of the red. Although he is thus
less ornamented than the female, yet in the larger size of his body, larger
canine teeth, more developed whiskers, more prominent superciliary ridges, he
follows the common rule of the male excelling the female.</p>
<p>I have now given all the cases known to me of a difference in colour between
the sexes of mammals. Some of these may be the result of variations confined to
one sex and transmitted to the same sex, without any good being gained, and
therefore without the aid of selection. We have instances of this with our
domesticated animals, as in the males of certain cats being rusty-red, whilst
the females are tortoise-shell coloured. Analogous cases occur in nature: Mr.
Bartlett has seen many black varieties of the jaguar, leopard, vulpine
phalanger, and wombat; and he is certain that all, or nearly all these animals,
were males. On the other hand, with wolves, foxes, and apparently American
squirrels, both sexes are occasionally born black. Hence it is quite possible
that with some mammals a difference in colour between the sexes, especially
when this is congenital, may simply be the result, without the aid of
selection, of the occurrence of one or more variations, which from the first
were sexually limited in their transmission. Nevertheless it is improbable that
the diversified, vivid, and contrasted colours of certain quadrupeds, for
instance, of the above monkeys and antelopes, can thus be accounted for. We
should bear in mind that these colours do not appear in the male at birth, but
only at or near maturity; and that unlike ordinary variations, they are lost if
the male be emasculated. It is on the whole probable that the strongly-marked
colours and other ornamental characters of male quadrupeds are beneficial to
them in their rivalry with other males, and have consequently been acquired
through sexual selection. This view is strengthened by the differences in
colour between the sexes occurring almost exclusively, as may be collected from
the previous details, in those groups and sub-groups of mammals which present
other and strongly-marked secondary sexual characters; these being likewise due
to sexual selection.</p>
<p>Quadrupeds manifestly take notice of colour. Sir S. Baker repeatedly observed
that the African elephant and rhinoceros attacked white or grey horses with
special fury. I have elsewhere shewn (32. The ‘Variation of Animals and
Plants under Domestication,’ 1868, vol. ii. pp. 102, 103.) that half-wild
horses apparently prefer to pair with those of the same colour, and that herds
of fallow-deer of different colours, though living together, have long kept
distinct. It is a more significant fact that a female zebra would not admit the
addresses of a male ass until he was painted so as to resemble a zebra, and
then, as John Hunter remarks, “she received him very readily. In this
curious fact, we have instinct excited by mere colour, which had so strong an
effect as to get the better of everything else. But the male did not require
this, the female being an animal somewhat similar to himself, was sufficient to
rouse him.” (33. ‘Essays and Observations,’ by J. Hunter,
edited by Owen, 1861, vol. i. p. 194.)</p>
<p>In an earlier chapter we have seen that the mental powers of the higher animals
do not differ in kind, though greatly in degree, from the corresponding powers
of man, especially of the lower and barbarous races; and it would appear that
even their taste for the beautiful is not widely different from that of the
Quadrumana. As the negro of Africa raises the flesh on his face into parallel
ridges “or cicatrices, high above the natural surface, which unsightly
deformities are considered great personal attractions” (34. Sir S. Baker,
‘The Nile Tributaries of Abyssinia,’ 1867.);—as negroes and
savages in many parts of the world paint their faces with red, blue, white, or
black bars,—so the male mandrill of Africa appears to have acquired his
deeply-furrowed and gaudily-coloured face from having been thus rendered
attractive to the female. No doubt it is to us a most grotesque notion that the
posterior end of the body should be coloured for the sake of ornament even more
brilliantly than the face; but this is not more strange than that the tails of
many birds should be especially decorated.</p>
<p>With mammals we do not at present possess any evidence that the males take
pains to display their charms before the female; and the elaborate manner in
which this is performed by male birds and other animals is the strongest
argument in favour of the belief that the females admire, or are excited by,
the ornaments and colours displayed before them. There is, however, a striking
parallelism between mammals and birds in all their secondary sexual characters,
namely in their weapons for fighting with rival males, in their ornamental
appendages, and in their colours. In both classes, when the male differs from
the female, the young of both sexes almost always resemble each other, and in a
large majority of cases resemble the adult female. In both classes the male
assumes the characters proper to his sex shortly before the age of
reproduction; and if emasculated at an early period, loses them. In both
classes the change of colour is sometimes seasonal, and the tints of the naked
parts sometimes become more vivid during the act of courtship. In both classes
the male is almost always more vividly or strongly coloured than the female,
and is ornamented with larger crests of hair or feathers, or other such
appendages. In a few exceptional cases the female in both classes is more
highly ornamented than the male. With many mammals, and at least in the case of
one bird, the male is more odoriferous than the female. In both classes the
voice of the male is more powerful than that of the female. Considering this
parallelism, there can be little doubt that the same cause, whatever it may be,
has acted on mammals and birds; and the result, as far as ornamental characters
are concerned, may be attributed, as it appears to me, to the long-continued
preference of the individuals of one sex for certain individuals of the
opposite sex, combined with their success in leaving a larger number of
offspring to inherit their superior attractions.</p>
<h3>EQUAL TRANSMISSION OF ORNAMENTAL CHARACTERS TO BOTH SEXES.</h3>
<p>With many birds, ornaments, which analogy leads us to believe were primarily
acquired by the males, have been transmitted equally, or almost equally, to
both sexes; and we may now enquire how far this view applies to mammals. With a
considerable number of species, especially of the smaller kinds, both sexes
have been coloured, independently of sexual selection, for the sake of
protection; but not, as far as I can judge, in so many cases, nor in so
striking a manner, as in most of the lower classes. Audubon remarks that he
often mistook the musk-rat (35. Fiber zibethicus, Audubon and Bachman,
‘The Quadrupeds of North America,’ 1846, p. 109.), whilst sitting
on the banks of a muddy stream, for a clod of earth, so complete was the
resemblance. The hare on her form is a familiar instance of concealment through
colour; yet this principle partly fails in a closely-allied species, the
rabbit, for when running to its burrow, it is made conspicuous to the
sportsman, and no doubt to all beasts of prey, by its upturned white tail. No
one doubts that the quadrupeds inhabiting snow-clad regions have been rendered
white to protect them from their enemies, or to favour their stealing on their
prey. In regions where snow never lies for long, a white coat would be
injurious; consequently, species of this colour are extremely rare in the
hotter parts of the world. It deserves notice that many quadrupeds inhabiting
moderately cold regions, although they do not assume a white winter dress,
become paler during this season; and this apparently is the direct result of
the conditions to which they have long been exposed. Pallas (36. ‘Novae
species Quadrupedum e Glirium ordine,’ 1778, p. 7. What I have called the
roe is the Capreolus sibiricus subecaudatus of Pallas.) states that in Siberia
a change of this nature occurs with the wolf, two species of Mustela, the
domestic horse, the Equus hemionus, the domestic cow, two species of antelopes,
the musk-deer, the roe, elk, and reindeer. The roe, for instance, has a red
summer and a greyish-white winter coat; and the latter may perhaps serve as a
protection to the animal whilst wandering through the leafless thickets,
sprinkled with snow and hoar-frost. If the above-named animals were gradually
to extend their range into regions perpetually covered with snow, their pale
winter-coats would probably be rendered through natural selection, whiter and
whiter, until they became as white as snow.</p>
<p>Mr. Reeks has given me a curious instance of an animal profiting by being
peculiarly coloured. He raised from fifty to sixty white and brown piebald
rabbits in a large walled orchard; and he had at the same time some similarly
coloured cats in his house. Such cats, as I have often noticed, are very
conspicuous during day; but as they used to lie in watch during the dusk at the
mouths of the burrows, the rabbits apparently did not distinguish them from
their parti-coloured brethren. The result was that, within eighteen months,
every one of these parti-coloured rabbits was destroyed; and there was evidence
that this was effected by the cats. Colour seems to be advantageous to another
animal, the skunk, in a manner of which we have had many instances in other
classes. No animal will voluntarily attack one of these creatures on account of
the dreadful odour which it emits when irritated; but during the dusk it would
not easily be recognised and might be attacked by a beast of prey. Hence it is,
as Mr. Belt believes (37. ‘The Naturalist in Nicaragua,’ p. 249.),
that the skunk is provided with a great white bushy tail, which serves as a
conspicuous warning.</p>
<p>[Fig. 70. Tragelaphus scriptus, male (from the Knowsley Menagerie).</p>
<p>Fig. 71. Damalis pygarga, male (from the Knowsley Menagerie).]</p>
<p>Although we must admit that many quadrupeds have received their present tints
either as a protection, or as an aid in procuring prey, yet with a host of
species, the colours are far too conspicuous and too singularly arranged to
allow us to suppose that they serve for these purposes. We may take as an
illustration certain antelopes; when we see the square white patch on the
throat, the white marks on the fetlocks, and the round black spots on the ears,
all more distinct in the male of the Portax picta, than in the
female;—when we see that the colours are more vivid, that the narrow
white lines on the flank and the broad white bar on the shoulder are more
distinct in the male Oreas derbyanus than in the female;—when we see a
similar difference between the sexes of the curiously-ornamented Tragelaphus
scriptus (Fig. 70),—we cannot believe that differences of this kind are
of any service to either sex in their daily habits of life. It seems a much
more probable conclusion that the various marks were first acquired by the
males and their colours intensified through sexual selection, and then
partially transferred to the females. If this view be admitted, there can be
little doubt that the equally singular colours and marks of many other
antelopes, though common to both sexes, have been gained and transmitted in a
like manner. Both sexes, for instance, of the koodoo (Strepsiceros kudu) (Fig.
64) have narrow white vertical lines on their hind flanks, and an elegant
angular white mark on their foreheads. Both sexes in the genus Damalis are very
oddly coloured; in D. pygarga the back and neck are purplish-red, shading on
the flanks into black; and these colours are abruptly separated from the white
belly and from a large white space on the buttocks; the head is still more
oddly coloured, a large oblong white mask, narrowly-edged with black, covers
the face up to the eyes (Fig. 71); there are three white stripes on the
forehead, and the ears are marked with white. The fawns of this species are of
a uniform pale yellowish-brown. In Damalis albifrons the colouring of the head
differs from that in the last species in a single white stripe replacing the
three stripes, and in the ears being almost wholly white. (38. See the fine
plates in A. Smith’s ‘Zoology of South Africa,’ and Dr.
Gray’s ‘Gleanings from the Menagerie of Knowsley.’) After
having studied to the best of my ability the sexual differences of animals
belonging to all classes, I cannot avoid the conclusion that the
curiously-arranged colours of many antelopes, though common to both sexes, are
the result of sexual selection primarily applied to the male.</p>
<p>The same conclusion may perhaps be extended to the tiger, one of the most
beautiful animals in the world, the sexes of which cannot be distinguished by
colour, even by the dealers in wild beasts. Mr. Wallace believes (39.
‘Westminster Review,’ July 1, 1867, p. 5.) that the striped coat of
the tiger “so assimilates with the vertical stems of the bamboo, as to
assist greatly in concealing him from his approaching prey.” But this
view does not appear to me satisfactory. We have some slight evidence that his
beauty may be due to sexual selection, for in two species of Felis the
analogous marks and colours are rather brighter in the male than in the female.
The zebra is conspicuously striped, and stripes cannot afford any protection in
the open plains of South Africa. Burchell (40. ‘Travels in South
Africa,’ 1824, vol. ii. p. 315.) in describing a herd says, “their
sleek ribs glistened in the sun, and the brightness and regularity of their
striped coats presented a picture of extraordinary beauty, in which probably
they are not surpassed by any other quadruped.” But as throughout the
whole group of the Equidae the sexes are identical in colour, we have here no
evidence of sexual selection. Nevertheless he who attributes the white and dark
vertical stripes on the flanks of various antelopes to this process, will
probably extend the same view to the Royal Tiger and beautiful Zebra.</p>
<p>We have seen in a former chapter that when young animals belonging to any class
follow nearly the same habits of life as their parents, and yet are coloured in
a different manner, it may be inferred that they have retained the colouring of
some ancient and extinct progenitor. In the family of pigs, and in the tapirs,
the young are marked with longitudinal stripes, and thus differ from all the
existing adult species in these two groups. With many kinds of deer the young
are marked with elegant white spots, of which their parents exhibit not a
trace. A graduated series can be followed from the axis deer, both sexes of
which at all ages and during all seasons are beautifully spotted (the male
being rather more strongly coloured than the female), to species in which
neither the old nor the young are spotted. I will specify some of the steps in
this series. The Mantchurian deer (Cervus mantchuricus) is spotted during the
whole year, but, as I have seen in the Zoological Gardens, the spots are much
plainer during the summer, when the general colour of the coat is lighter, than
during the winter, when the general colour is darker and the horns are fully
developed. In the hog-deer (Hyelaphus porcinus) the spots are extremely
conspicuous during the summer when the coat is reddish-brown, but quite
disappear during the winter when the coat is brown. (41. Dr. Gray,
‘Gleanings from the Menagerie of Knowsley,’ p. 64. Mr. Blyth, in
speaking (‘Land and Water,’ 1869, p. 42) of the hog-deer of Ceylon,
says it is more brightly spotted with white than the common hog-deer, at the
season when it renews its horns.) In both these species the young are spotted.
In the Virginian deer the young are likewise spotted, and about five per cent.
of the adult animals living in Judge Caton’s park, as I am informed by
him, temporarily exhibit at the period when the red summer coat is being
replaced by the bluish winter coat, a row of spots on each flank, which are
always the same in number, though very variable in distinctness. From this
condition there is but a very small step to the complete absence of spots in
the adults at all seasons; and, lastly, to their absence at all ages and
seasons, as occurs with certain species. From the existence of this perfect
series, and more especially from the fawns of so many species being spotted, we
may conclude that the now living members of the deer family are the descendants
of some ancient species which, like the axis deer, was spotted at all ages and
seasons. A still more ancient progenitor probably somewhat resembled the
Hyomoschus aquaticus—for this animal is spotted, and the hornless males
have large exserted canine teeth, of which some few true deer still retain
rudiments. Hyomoschus, also, offers one of those interesting cases of a form
linking together two groups, for it is intermediate in certain osteological
characters between the pachyderms and ruminants, which were formerly thought to
be quite distinct. (42. Falconer and Cautley, ‘Proc. Geolog. Soc.’
1843; and Falconer’s ‘Pal. Memoirs,’ vol. i. p. 196.)</p>
<p>A curious difficulty here arises. If we admit that coloured spots and stripes
were first acquired as ornaments, how comes it that so many existing deer, the
descendants of an aboriginally spotted animal, and all the species of pigs and
tapirs, the descendants of an aboriginally striped animal, have lost in their
adult state their former ornaments? I cannot satisfactorily answer this
question. We may feel almost sure that the spots and stripes disappeared at or
near maturity in the progenitors of our existing species, so that they were
still retained by the young; and, owing to the law of inheritance at
corresponding ages, were transmitted to the young of all succeeding
generations. It may have been a great advantage to the lion and puma, from the
open nature of their usual haunts, to have lost their stripes, and to have been
thus rendered less conspicuous to their prey; and if the successive variations,
by which this end was gained, occurred rather late in life, the young would
have retained their stripes, as is now the case. As to deer, pigs, and tapirs,
Fritz Müller has suggested to me that these animals, by the removal of their
spots or stripes through natural selection, would have been less easily seen by
their enemies; and that they would have especially required this protection, as
soon as the carnivora increased in size and number during the tertiary periods.
This may be the true explanation, but it is rather strange that the young
should not have been thus protected, and still more so that the adults of some
species should have retained their spots, either partially or completely,
during part of the year. We know that, when the domestic ass varies and becomes
reddish-brown, grey, or black, the stripes on the shoulders and even on the
spine frequently disappear, though we cannot explain the cause. Very few
horses, except dun-coloured kinds, have stripes on any part of their bodies,
yet we have good reason to believe that the aboriginal horse was striped on the
legs and spine, and probably on the shoulders. (43. The ‘Variation of
Animals and Plants under Domestication,’ 1868, vol. i. pp. 61-64.) Hence
the disappearance of the spots and stripes in our adult existing deer, pigs,
and tapirs, may be due to a change in the general colour of their coats; but
whether this change was effected through sexual or natural selection, or was
due to the direct action of the conditions of life, or to some other unknown
cause, it is impossible to decide. An observation made by Mr. Sclater well
illustrates our ignorance of the laws which regulate the appearance and
disappearance of stripes; the species of Asinus which inhabit the Asiatic
continent are destitute of stripes, not having even the cross shoulder-stripe,
whilst those which inhabit Africa are conspicuously striped, with the partial
exception of A. taeniopus, which has only the cross shoulder-stripe and
generally some faint bars on the legs; and this species inhabits the almost
intermediate region of Upper Egypt and Abyssinia. (44. ‘Proc. Zool.
Soc.’ 1862, p. 164. See, also, Dr. Hartmann, ‘Ann. d. Landw.’
Bd. xliii. s. 222.)</p>
<h3>QUADRUMANA.</h3>
<p>[Fig. 72. Head of Semnopithecus rubicundus. This and the following figures
(from Prof. Gervais) are given to shew the odd arrangement and development of
the hair on the head.</p>
<p>Fig. 73. Head of Semnopithecus comatus.</p>
<p>Fig. 74. Head of Cebus capucinus.</p>
<p>Fig. 75. Head of Ateles marginatus.</p>
<p>Fig. 76. Head of Cebus vellerosus.]</p>
<p>Before we conclude, it will be well to add a few remarks on the ornaments of
monkeys. In most of the species the sexes resemble each other in colour, but in
some, as we have seen, the males differ from the females, especially in the
colour of the naked parts of the skin, in the development of the beard,
whiskers, and mane. Many species are coloured either in so extraordinary or so
beautiful a manner, and are furnished with such curious and elegant crests of
hair, that we can hardly avoid looking at these characters as having been
gained for the sake of ornament. The accompanying figures (Figs. 72 to 76)
serve to shew the arrangement of the hair on the face and head in several
species. It is scarcely conceivable that these crests of hair, and the strongly
contrasted colours of the fur and skin, can be the result of mere variability
without the aid of selection; and it is inconceivable that they can be of use
in any ordinary way to these animals. If so, they have probably been gained
through sexual selection, though transmitted equally, or almost equally, to
both sexes. With many of the Quadrumana, we have additional evidence of the
action of sexual selection in the greater size and strength of the males, and
in the greater development of their canine teeth, in comparison with the
females.</p>
<p>[Fig. 77. Cercopithecus petaurista (from Brehm).]</p>
<p>A few instances will suffice of the strange manner in which both sexes of some
species are coloured, and of the beauty of others. The face of the
Cercopithecus petaurista (Fig. 77) is black, the whiskers and beard being
white, with a defined, round, white spot on the nose, covered with short white
hair, which gives to the animal an almost ludicrous aspect. The Semnopithecus
frontatus likewise has a blackish face with a long black beard, and a large
naked spot on the forehead of a bluish-white colour. The face of Macacus
lasiotus is dirty flesh-coloured, with a defined red spot on each cheek. The
appearance of Cercocebus aethiops is grotesque, with its black face, white
whiskers and collar, chestnut head, and a large naked white spot over each
eyelid. In very many species, the beard, whiskers, and crests of hair round the
face are of a different colour from the rest of the head, and when different,
are always of a lighter tint (45. I observed this fact in the Zoological
Gardens; and many cases may be seen in the coloured plates in Geoffroy
St.-Hilaire and F. Cuvier, ‘Histoire Nat. des Mammifères,’ tom. i.
1824.), being often pure white, sometimes bright yellow, or reddish. The whole
face of the South American Brachyurus calvus is of a “glowing scarlet
hue”; but this colour does not appear until the animal is nearly mature.
(46. Bates, ‘The Naturalist on the Amazons,’ 1863, vol. ii. p.
310.) The naked skin of the face differs wonderfully in colour in the various
species. It is often brown or flesh-colour, with parts perfectly white, and
often as black as that of the most sooty negro. In the Brachyurus the scarlet
tint is brighter than that of the most blushing Caucasian damsel. It is
sometimes more distinctly orange than in any Mongolian, and in several species
it is blue, passing into violet or grey. In all the species known to Mr.
Bartlett, in which the adults of both sexes have strongly-coloured faces, the
colours are dull or absent during early youth. This likewise holds good with
the mandrill and Rhesus, in which the face and the posterior parts of the body
are brilliantly coloured in one sex alone. In these latter cases we have reason
to believe that the colours were acquired through sexual selection; and we are
naturally led to extend the same view to the foregoing species, though both
sexes when adult have their faces coloured in the same manner.</p>
<p>[Fig. 78. Cercopithecus diana (from Brehm).]</p>
<p>Although many kinds of monkeys are far from beautiful according to our taste,
other species are universally admired for their elegant appearance and bright
colours. The Semnopithecus nemaeus, though peculiarly coloured, is described as
extremely pretty; the orange-tinted face is surrounded by long whiskers of
glossy whiteness, with a line of chestnut-red over the eyebrows; the fur on the
back is of a delicate grey, with a square patch on the loins, the tail and the
fore-arms being of a pure white; a gorget of chestnut surmounts the chest; the
thighs are black, with the legs chestnut-red. I will mention only two other
monkeys for their beauty; and I have selected these as presenting slight sexual
differences in colour, which renders it in some degree probable that both sexes
owe their elegant appearance to sexual selection. In the moustache-monkey
(Cercopithecus cephus) the general colour of the fur is mottled-greenish with
the throat white; in the male the end of the tail is chestnut, but the face is
the most ornamented part, the skin being chiefly bluish-grey, shading into a
blackish tint beneath the eyes, with the upper lip of a delicate blue, clothed
on the lower edge with a thin black moustache; the whiskers are
orange-coloured, with the upper part black, forming a band which extends
backwards to the ears, the latter being clothed with whitish hairs. In the
Zoological Society’s Gardens I have often overheard visitors admiring the
beauty of another monkey, deservedly called Cercopithecus diana (Fig. 78); the
general colour of the fur is grey; the chest and inner surface of the forelegs
are white; a large triangular defined space on the hinder part of the back is
rich chestnut; in the male the inner sides of the thighs and the abdomen are
delicate fawn-coloured, and the top of the head is black; the face and ears are
intensely black, contrasting finely with a white transverse crest over the
eyebrows and a long white peaked beard, of which the basal portion is black.
(47. I have seen most of the above monkeys in the Zoological Society’s
Gardens. The description of the Semnopithecus nemaeus is taken from Mr. W.C.
Martin’s ‘Natural History of Mammalia,’ 1841, p. 460; see
also pp. 475, 523.)</p>
<p>In these and many other monkeys, the beauty and singular arrangement of their
colours, and still more the diversified and elegant arrangement of the crests
and tufts of hair on their heads, force the conviction on my mind that these
characters have been acquired through sexual selection exclusively as
ornaments.</p>
<h3>A SUMMARY.</h3>
<p>The law of battle for the possession of the female appears to prevail
throughout the whole great class of mammals. Most naturalists will admit that
the greater size, strength, courage, and pugnacity of the male, his special
weapons of offence, as well as his special means of defence, have been acquired
or modified through that form of selection which I have called sexual. This
does not depend on any superiority in the general struggle for life, but on
certain individuals of one sex, generally the male, being successful in
conquering other males, and leaving a larger number of offspring to inherit
their superiority than do the less successful males.</p>
<p>There is another and more peaceful kind of contest, in which the males
endeavour to excite or allure the females by various charms. This is probably
carried on in some cases by the powerful odours emitted by the males during the
breeding-season; the odoriferous glands having been acquired through sexual
selection. Whether the same view can be extended to the voice is doubtful, for
the vocal organs of the males must have been strengthened by use during
maturity, under the powerful excitements of love, jealousy or rage, and will
consequently have been transmitted to the same sex. Various crests, tufts, and
mantles of hair, which are either confined to the male, or are more developed
in this sex than in the female, seem in most cases to be merely ornamental,
though they sometimes serve as a defence against rival males. There is even
reason to suspect that the branching horns of stags, and the elegant horns of
certain antelopes, though properly serving as weapons of offence or defence,
have been partly modified for ornament.</p>
<p>When the male differs in colour from the female, he generally exhibits darker
and more strongly-contrasted tints. We do not in this class meet with the
splendid red, blue, yellow, and green tints, so common with male birds and many
other animals. The naked parts, however, of certain Quadrumana must be
excepted; for such parts, often oddly situated, are brilliantly coloured in
some species. The colours of the male in other cases may be due to simple
variation, without the aid of selection. But when the colours are diversified
and strongly pronounced, when they are not developed until near maturity, and
when they are lost after emasculation, we can hardly avoid the conclusion that
they have been acquired through sexual selection for the sake of ornament, and
have been transmitted exclusively, or almost exclusively, to the same sex. When
both sexes are coloured in the same manner, and the colours are conspicuous or
curiously arranged, without being of the least apparent use as a protection,
and especially when they are associated with various other ornamental
appendages, we are led by analogy to the same conclusion, namely, that they
have been acquired through sexual selection, although transmitted to both
sexes. That conspicuous and diversified colours, whether confined to the males
or common to both sexes, are as a general rule associated in the same groups
and sub-groups with other secondary sexual characters serving for war or for
ornament, will be found to hold good, if we look back to the various cases
given in this and the last chapter.</p>
<p>The law of the equal transmission of characters to both sexes, as far as colour
and other ornaments are concerned, has prevailed far more extensively with
mammals than with birds; but weapons, such as horns and tusks, have often been
transmitted either exclusively or much more perfectly to the males than to the
females. This is surprising, for, as the males generally use their weapons for
defence against enemies of all kinds, their weapons would have been of service
to the females. As far as we can see, their absence in this sex can be
accounted for only by the form of inheritance which has prevailed. Finally,
with quadrupeds the contest between the individuals of the same sex, whether
peaceful or bloody, has, with the rarest exceptions, been confined to the
males; so that the latter have been modified through sexual selection, far more
commonly than the females, either for fighting with each other or for alluring
the opposite sex.</p>
<!--end chapter-->
<SPAN name="endofbook"></SPAN>
<div style="break-after:column;"></div><br />